Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Perceived risks and benefits of medical remedies and procedures: What do men and women think?

Abstract

Purpose:

Are men more inclined to assume risks than women? The answer to this question is immediately relevant to various economic issues, including human behavior concerning medical remedies and procedures consumption. This paper aims to identify the perceived benefits of the consumption of medications centered on pharmaceutical risk assessment.

Originality/value:

The literature documents that risks and benefits can be considered the main trade-offs in consumer decision-making, including the pharmaceutical industry. It is a current theme with a high potential for social impact due to the many identified cases of Covid-19. In developing countries, most medication is used by self-medicating individuals, as access to health care can be insufficient. Self-medication is a potentially greater problem, as in this case, medication usage often follows advice from friends, relatives, and neighbors. Yet, there is little empirical evidence regarding this problem in the context of emerging countries.

Design/methodology/approach:

A survey of over 1,000 subjects using an extended regression model (ERM) in one of the biggest countries in the world, Brazil.

Findings:

The results reported that women perceive more risks and benefits from medication usage and medical procedures. The perception of the benefits of medications can be explained by the following dimensions: risk (personal injury, risk alertness, and risk awareness), gender, regular usage, and the benefits of medical procedures. Regarding risk dimensions, the lower the perceptions of personal injury and the higher the perceptions of risk alertness and risk awareness, the greater the perceived benefits of medication usage.

Keywords:
benefits; risks; gender; drugs; emerging countries

Resumo

Objetivo:

Os homens estão mais inclinados a assumir riscos do que as mulheres? A resposta a essa pergunta é de relevância imediata para várias questões econômicas, incluindo o comportamento humano em relação ao consumo de medicamentos e procedimentos médicos. Este trabalho tem como objetivo identificar os benefícios percebidos do consumo de medicamentos, centrado na avaliação do risco farmacêutico.

Originalidade/valor:

A literatura documenta que riscos e benefícios podem ser considerados os principais trade-offs na tomada de decisão do consumidor, inclusive na indústria farmacêutica. É um tema atual com alto potencial de impacto social por causa dos muitos casos identificados de Covid-19. Nos países em desenvolvimento, a maioria dos medicamentos é utilizada por indivíduos que se automedicam, pois o acesso aos cuidados de saúde pode ser insuficiente. A automedicação é um problema potencialmente maior, pois nesse caso o uso de medicamentos geralmente segue o conselho de amigos, parentes e vizinhos. No entanto, há pouca evidência empírica sobre esse problema no contexto dos países emergentes.

Design/metodologia/abordagem:

Trata-se de uma pesquisa com mais de mil indivíduos em que se utilizou um modelo de regressão estendido (MRE) em um dos maiores países do mundo, o Brasil.

Resultados:

De acordo com os resultados obtidos, as mulheres percebem mais riscos e benefícios do uso de medicamentos e procedimentos médicos. A percepção dos benefícios dos medicamentos pode ser explicada pelas seguintes dimensões: risco (danos pessoais, alerta de risco e conhecimento do risco), gênero, uso regular e benefícios dos procedimentos médicos. Em relação às dimensões de risco, quanto menor a percepção de danos pessoais e quanto maior a percepção de alerta e conhecimento de risco, maiores os benefícios percebidos do uso de medicamentos.

Palavras-chave:
benefícios; riscos; gênero; medicamentos; países emergentes

INTRODUCTION

Risk is a part of people’s lives in many different contexts. With technological advances, societies are increasingly challenged to make decisions about the risks of new technologies and products/services (Beck, 1992Beck, U. (1992). Risk society: Towards a new modernity. University of Munich.; Bodemer & Gaissmaier, 2015Bodemer, N., & Gaissmaier, W. (2015). Risk perception. In H. Cho, T. Reimer, & K. McComas (Eds.), The Sage handbook of risk communication. Sage.; Jenkins et al., 2021Jenkins, S. C., Harris, A. J. L., & Osman, M. (2021). What drives risk perceptions? Revisiting public perceptions of food hazards associated with production and consumption. Journal of Risk Research, 24(11), 1450–1464. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1871057
https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.18...
). As it is neither possible nor desirable to avoid these risks, people should be able to deal with them. The problem arises when some risks, particularly those that can cause reactions (medications), are underestimated and their consequences are ignored. In the medical industry, perceived risks are related to the pharmacological properties of medications and patient consumption (Letzel, 1989Letzel, H. (1989). Statistics in drug risk research: The background of pharmacoepidemiology. In B. Horisberger, R. Dinkel (Eds.), The perception and management of drug safety risks. Springer.). It is a current theme with a high potential for social impact due to the many identified cases of the novel coronavirus disease (designated as 2019-nCoV by the World Health Organization). Empirical investigations in this area are important to understand and assist in stopping these outbreaks.

From a constructivist viewpoint, the perception of medications’ risks and benefits, and consequently the decision of whether and how to use them, arises from a complex interaction of affective and cognitive processes, strongly influenced by individual, cultural, and social characteristics (Burgess, 2015Burgess, A. (2015). Social construction of risk. In H. Cho, T. Reimer, K. McComas. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of risk communication. Sage.; Gardner & Jones, 2011Gardner, G. E., & Jones, M. G. (2011). Science instructors’ perceptions of the risks of biotechnology: Implications for science education. Research in Science Education, 41(5), 711–738. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-010-9187-0
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-010-9187-...
). Among individual characteristics, gender has stood out for influencing perceptions of risk and benefit in the consumption of medical products (Axon et al., 2009Axon, R. N., Bradford, W. D., & Egan, B. M. (2009). The role of individual time preferences in health behaviors among hypertensive adults: A pilot study. Journal of the American Society of Hypertension, 3(1), 35–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jash.2008.08.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jash.2008.08.0...
; Bradford, 2010Bradford, W. D. (2010). The association between individual time preferences and health maintenance habits. Medical Decision Making, 30(1), 99–112. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X09342276
https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X09342276...
; Brandt & Dickinson, 2013Brandt, S., & Dickinson, B. (2013). Time and risk preferences and the use of asthma controller medication. Pediatrics, 131(4), e1204–e1210. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2982
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2982...
; Chapman & Coups, 1999Chapman, G. B., & Coups, E. J. (1999). Predictors of influenza vaccine acceptance among healthy adults. Preventive Medicine, 29(4), 249–262. https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1999.0535
https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1999.0535...
; Filia et al., 2014Filia, S. L., Baker, A. L., Gurvich, C. T., Richmond, R., & Kulkarni, J. (2014). The perceived risks and benefits of quitting in smokers diagnosed with severe mental illness participating in a smoking cessation intervention: Gender differences and comparison to smokers without mental illness. Drug and Alcohol Review, 33(1), 78–85. https://doi.org/10.1111/dar.12091
https://doi.org/10.1111/dar.12091...
; Finucane et al., 2000Finucane, M. L., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Flynn, J., & Satterfield, T. A. (2000). Gender, race, and perceived risk: The “white male” effect. Health, Risk & Society, 2(2), 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1080/713670162
https://doi.org/10.1080/713670162...
; Mahalik et al., 2015Mahalik, J. R., Lombardi, C. M., Sims, J., Coley, R. L., & Lynch, A. D. (2015). Gender, male-typicality, and social norms predicting adolescent alcohol intoxication and marijuana use. Social Science & Medicine, 143, 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.08.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015...
; Picone et al., 2004Picone, G., Sloan, F., & Taylor, D. (2004). Effects of risk and time preference and expected longevity on demand for medical tests. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 28(1), 39–53. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:RISK.0000009435.11390.23
https://doi.org/10.1023/B:RISK.000000943...
), as women take more medications than men (Green, 2006Green, C. (2006). Gender and use of substance abuse treatment services. Alcohol Research and Health, 29(1), 55–62.; Kandall, 2010Kandall, S. R. (2010). Women and drug addiction: A historical perspective. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 29(2), 117–126. https://doi.org/10.1080/10550881003684491
https://doi.org/10.1080/1055088100368449...
). Hypochondriac (fear of serious illness) concerns and attitudes are also more common in women (Hernandez & Kellner, 1992Hernandez, J., & Kellner, R. (1992). Hypochondriacal concerns and attitudes toward illness in males and females. The International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 22(3), 251–263. https://doi.org/10.2190/W9KG-6HU9-5QJX-NW76
https://doi.org/10.2190/W9KG-6HU9-5QJX-N...
) and can trigger other health problems like anxiety and depression (Fallon et al., 2012Fallon, B. A., Harper, K. M., Landa, A., Pavlicova, M., Schneier, F. R., Carson, A., Harding, K., Keegan, K., Schwartz, T., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2012). Personality disorders in hypochondriasis: Prevalence and comparison with two anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics, 53(6), 566–574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psym.2012.02.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psym.2012.02.0...
; Rodriguez-Besteiro et al., 2021Rodriguez-Besteiro, S., Tornero-Aguilera, J. F., Fernández-Lucas, J., & Clemente-Suárez, V. J. (2021). Gender differences in the Covid-19 pandemic risk perception, psychology and behaviors of spanish university students. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(8), 3908. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083908
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083908...
).

The increase in medication consumption by women can be associated with social and/or occupational barriers faced by this gender. It can be triggered by loneliness, stress, tiredness, dietary issues, low self-esteem, and body image problems (Greenfield et al., 2010Greenfield, S. F., Back, S. E., Lawson, K., & Brady, K. T. (2010). Substance abuse in women. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33(2), 339–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2010.01.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2010.01.00...
). In stress-related disorders, women are at higher risk of depression and anxiety (Rodriguez-Besteiro et al., 2021Rodriguez-Besteiro, S., Tornero-Aguilera, J. F., Fernández-Lucas, J., & Clemente-Suárez, V. J. (2021). Gender differences in the Covid-19 pandemic risk perception, psychology and behaviors of spanish university students. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(8), 3908. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083908
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083908...
), and men are at higher risk of alcohol-related disorders (Chaplin et al., 2008Chaplin, T. M., Hong, K., Bergquist, K., & Sinha, R. (2008). Gender differences in response to emotional stress: An assessment across subjective, behavioral, and physiological domains and relations to alcohol craving. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32(7), 1242–1250. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00679.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008...
; Levy et al., 2021Levy, I., Cohen-Louck, K., & Bonny-Noach, H. (2021). Gender, employment, and continuous pandemic as predictors of alcohol and drug consumption during the Covid-19. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 228, 109029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.109029
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.202...
). Another aspect is related to women’s propensity to seek help (Hunt et al., 2011Hunt, K., Adamson, J., Hewitt, C., & Nazareth, I. (2011). Do women consult more than men? A review of gender and consultation for back pain and headache. Journal of Health Services Research and Policy, 16(2), 108–117. https://doi.org/10.1258/jhsrp.2010.009131
https://doi.org/10.1258/jhsrp.2010.00913...
). Biological issues also come into this discussion, justifying that sex and hormonal differences between the genders cause them to have different reactions to medications. Women experience more pain than men and therefore need more medication, especially in cases such as migraines, fibromyalgia, and osteoarthritis, among others (Kandall, 2010Kandall, S. R. (2010). Women and drug addiction: A historical perspective. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 29(2), 117–126. https://doi.org/10.1080/10550881003684491
https://doi.org/10.1080/1055088100368449...
).

Thus, this study aims to understand men’s and women’s perceived risks and benefits of consuming medications and medical procedures in a developing country. In these countries, medications can account for 30% to 40% of health-related spending, yet average medicine availability does not reach 35% (World Health Organization – WHO, 2009World Health Organization (2009). Continuity and change: Implementing the third WHO Medicines Strategy 2008-2013.). Only 16% of the world’s population accounts for 78% of pharmaceutical spending. Another 71% of the same population, living in 78 countries with low or medium per capita income, account for only 11% of pharmaceutical spending. The spending discrepancy is even greater per capita: US$ 7.61 in low-income countries and US$ 431.60 in high-income countries (Kaplan & Mathers, 2011Kaplan, W., & Mathers, C. (2011). Global health trends: Global burden of disease and pharmaceutical needs. In World Health Organization (Ed.), The world medicines situation 2011.).

In developing countries, most medication is used by self-medicating individuals, as access to health care can be insufficient (Nguyen et al., 2013Nguyen, K. V., Do, N. T. T., Chandna, A., Nguyen, T. V., Pham, C. V., Doan, P. M., Nguyen, A. Q., Nguyen, C. K. T., Larsson, M., Escalante, S., Olowokure, B., Laxminarayan, R., Gelband, H., Horby, P., Ngo, H. B. T., Hoang, M. T., Farrar, J., Hien, T. T., & Wertheim, H. F. (2013). Antibiotic use and resistance in emerging economies: A situation analysis for Viet Nam. BMC Public Health, 13(1), 1158. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-1158
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-115...
). Self-medication is a potentially greater problem, as in this case, medication usage often follows advice from friends, relatives, and neighbors (Grigoryan et al., 2006Grigoryan, L., Haaijer-Ruskamp, F. M., Burgerhof, J. G. M., Mechtler, R., Deschepper, R., Tambic-Andrasevic, A., Andrajati, R., Monnet, D. L., Cunney, R., Di Matteo, A., Edelstein, H., Valinteliene, R., Alkerwi, A., Scicluna, E. A., Grzesiowski, P., Bara, A. C., Tesar, T., Cizman, M., Campos, J. … Birkin, J. (2006). Self-medication with antimicrobial drugs in Europe. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 12(3), 452–459. https://doi.org/10.3201/eid1203.050992
https://doi.org/10.3201/eid1203.050992...
). People frequently keep leftover medications in their homes and reuse them or donate them to people in need (Hardon et al., 2004Hardon, A., Hodgkin, C., & Fresle, D. (2004). How to investigate the use of medicines by consumers. World Health Organization, University of Amsterdam.). Still, some demographic characteristics are distinct from developed countries and can be decisive for properly perceiving risks and benefits (Krewski et al., 2006Krewski, D., Lemyre, L., Turner, M. C., Lee, J. E. C., Dallaire, C., Bouchard, L., Brand, K., Mercier, P. (2006). Public perception of population health risks in Canada: Health hazards and sources of information. Human and Ecological Risk Assessment, 12(4), 626–644. https://doi.org/10.1080/10807030600561832
https://doi.org/10.1080/1080703060056183...
). The lower average level of formal education can limit the ability to make assertive consumption decisions. Additionally, population aging and increased life expectancy increase medical care and medication consumption, requiring adjustments to the public health system (Cuevas et al., 2017Cuevas, A., Karpowicz, I., Granados, C., & Soto, M. (2017). Fiscal challenges of population aging in Brazil. IMF Working Papers, 17(99). https://doi.org/10.5089/9781475595550.001
https://doi.org/10.5089/9781475595550.00...
).

Regarding psychological issues, the increase in the number of individuals with anxiety and depression, representative suicide rates (O’Connor et al., 2017O’Connor, S. S., Comtois, K. A., Atkins, D. C., & Kerbrat, A. H. (2017). Examining the impact of suicide attempt function and perceived effectiveness in predicting reattempt for emergency medicine patients. Behavior Therapy, 48(1), 45–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.05.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.05.0...
), and low mood levels (Bolton et al., 2009Bolton, J. M., Robinson, J., & Sareen, J. (2009). Self-medication of mood disorders with alcohol and drugs in the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Affective Disorders, 115(3), 367–375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.00...
) can trigger the consumption of medications, and these aspects are observed in emerging countries. In the current research, Brazil was taken to represent emerging countries. The study was conducted in the metropolitan region of São Paulo, with the largest estimated population, 45,094,866 inhabitants (Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística, 2017Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística (2017). Estimativas da população residente nos municípios brasileiros com base em 1º de julho de 2017.); it is a representative commercial and industrial center of the country, covering a wide range of socioeconomic groups (Baer, 2014Baer, W. (2014). The Brazilian economy: Growth & development. Lynne Rienner.; Vieira & Ford, 1996Vieira, E. M., & Ford, N. J. (1996). The provision of female sterilization in São Paulo, Brazil: A study among low income women. Social Science and Medicine, 42(10), 1427–1432. https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(95)00291-X
https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(95)002...
) and about 33% of the Brazilian gross domestic product (Lucas et al., 2017Lucas, E. C., Mendes-Da-Silva, W., & Lyons, A. C. (2017). Gender differences in attitudes towards driving and demand for private Insurance: Evidence from middle class drivers. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 47, 72–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2017.04.022
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2017.04.02...
).

This paper contributes in different ways. First is the importance of individual characteristics, such as gender, to understand risk perception and benefits. The influence of individual characteristics on risk-taking has been extensively discussed in the literature (Beyer et al., 2015Beyer, A. R., Fasolo, B., De Graeff, P. A., & Hillege, H. L. (2015). Risk attitudes and personality traits predict perceptions of benefits and risks for medicinal products: A field study of European medical assessors. Value in Health, 18(1), 91–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2014.10.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2014.10.0...
). However, allowing comparisons to be made across populations from different nations and analyzing the risk/return relationship in medication use is different. In practical and social aspects, research can contribute to health management, especially after the pandemic. During pandemics, uncertainty, communication, and perception risk increase due to high infection rates, significant morbidity, lack of therapeutic measures, and rapid increases in cases. Changes in risk perception and poor risk communication can induce some behaviors that can lead to a lack of medications and personal protective equipment (Abrams & Greenhawt, 2020Abrams, E. M., & Greenhawt, M. (2020). Risk communication during Covid-19. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology: In Practice, 8(6), 1791–1794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaip.2020.04.012
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaip.2020.04.0...
) and rejection of vaccines and new drugs (Paudel et al., 2021Paudel, S., Palaian, S., Shankar, P. R., & Subedi, N. (2021). Risk perception and hesitancy toward Covid-19 vaccination among healthcare workers and staff at a medical college in Nepal. Risk Management and Healthcare Policy, 14, 2253–2261. https://doi.org/10.2147/RMHP.S310289
https://doi.org/10.2147/RMHP.S310289...
).

Therefore, the contributions at the health management level will help design better forms of communication with patients and the public, assess the impact of new drug problems, and ensure safe and effective medication usage (Slovic et al., 2007Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
). Besides, they will highlight the relationships between risk dimensions and the importance of knowledge about diseases for perceived risk (London & Robles, 2000London, A. S., & Robles, A. (2000). The co-occurrence of correct and incorrect HIV transmission knowledge and perceived risk for HIV among women of childbearing age in El Salvador. Social Science & Medicine, 51(8), 1267–1278. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00044-7
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00...
). Particularly, this research relates to understanding the perceived benefits of medication usage, which is considered as complex as risk perception (Beyer et al., 2015Beyer, A. R., Fasolo, B., De Graeff, P. A., & Hillege, H. L. (2015). Risk attitudes and personality traits predict perceptions of benefits and risks for medicinal products: A field study of European medical assessors. Value in Health, 18(1), 91–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2014.10.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2014.10.0...
).

BACKGROUND

Perception of risks and benefits

Risks are entangled with people’s lives, and risk management plays an important role in modern society (Fischhoff, 1983Fischhoff, B. (1983). “Acceptable risk”: The case of nuclear power. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 2(4), 559–575. https://doi.org/10.2307/3323574
https://doi.org/10.2307/3323574...
; Hopkin, 2017Hopkin, P. (2017). Fundamentals of risk management: Understanding, evaluating and implementing effective risk management. Kogan Page.). In this paper, the constructivist paradigm is adopted, according to which beliefs, attitudes, judgments, forms of information processing, values, evaluations of cost-benefit balance, feelings, familiarity with emotional and affective risk, and the framing of media reports are all factors that are weighed in generating risk and benefit perception (Gardner & Jones, 2011Gardner, G. E., & Jones, M. G. (2011). Science instructors’ perceptions of the risks of biotechnology: Implications for science education. Research in Science Education, 41(5), 711–738. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-010-9187-0
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-010-9187-...
). Hence, perception is a personal assessment of risk in a decision situation (Sitkin & Pablo, 1992Sitkin, S. B., & Pablo, A. L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the determinants of risk behavior. Academy of Management Review, 17(1), 9–38. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1992.4279564
https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1992.4279564...
) consisting of many factors (Olsen, 2001Olsen, R. A. (2001). Behavioral finance as science: Implications from the research of Paul Slovic. Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets, 2(3), 157–159. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327760JPFM0203_5
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327760JPFM020...
), including observable characteristics such as age, income, and gender (Betz et al., 2002Betz, N. E., Weber, E. U., & Blais, A.-R. (2002). A domain-specific riskattitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263–290. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414
https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414...
; Breuer et al., 2017Breuer, W., Kollath, J., Salzmann, A., & Nitzsch, R. (2017). The assessment of risk behavior: A cross-cultural analysis using the DOSPERT scale. In I. Venezia (Eds.), Behavioral finance: Where do investors’ biases come from? World Scientific Publishing; Chapple & Johnson, 2007Chapple, C. L., & Johnson, K. A. (2007). Gender differences in impulsivity. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 5(3), 221–234. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541204007301286
https://doi.org/10.1177/1541204007301286...
; Galizzi et al., 2016Galizzi, M. M., Miraldo, M., Stavropoulou, C., & Pol, M. van der (2016). Doctor–patient differences in risk and time preferences: A field experiment. Journal of Health Economics, 50, 171–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2016.10.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2016....
), as well as cultural and social characteristics (Burgess, 2015Burgess, A. (2015). Social construction of risk. In H. Cho, T. Reimer, K. McComas. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of risk communication. Sage.; Gardner & Jones, 2011Gardner, G. E., & Jones, M. G. (2011). Science instructors’ perceptions of the risks of biotechnology: Implications for science education. Research in Science Education, 41(5), 711–738. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-010-9187-0
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-010-9187-...
).

In the constructivist understanding, there are several models for measuring risk perception. Particularly, there are the economic method and the psychological paradigm, which have in common the rejection of the existence of a risk formula (Hansen & Hammann, 2017Hansen, J., & Hammann, M. (2017). Risk in science instruction: The realist and constructivist paradigms of risk. Science and Education, 26(7–9), 749–775. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-017-9923-1
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-017-9923-...
). The psychological paradigm focuses on understanding how preferences and heuristics explain risk perception personally and predicting people’s reactions to risks (Kahneman & Tversky, 1972Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 430–454. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(72)90016-3
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(72)900...
; Renn, 2008Renn, O. (2008). White paper on risk governance: Toward an integrative framework. In O. Renn, K. Walker (Eds.), Global risk governance: Concept and practice using the IRGC framework. Springer.; Simon, 1955Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99–118. https://doi.org/10.2307/1884852
https://doi.org/10.2307/1884852...
). One of the most influential methods used to measure risk perception in this paradigm is psychometric (Fischhoff et al., 1978Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., Lichtenstein, S., Read, S., & Combs, B. (1978). How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sciences, 9(2), 127–152. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00143739
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00143739...
).

The psychometric method implements a psychophysical scale to measure the perceived risks of various technologies, products, and activities (Slovic, 1987Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science, 236(4799), 280–285. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507...
). It identifies risk characteristics that contribute to the formation of risk perception (Slovic et al., 1982Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982). Why study risk perception? Risk Analysis, 2(2), 83–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1982.tb01369.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1982...
). Among the different risk dimensions are risk awareness, risk control, fear perception, and alertness risk, among others (Fischhoff et al., 1978Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., Lichtenstein, S., Read, S., & Combs, B. (1978). How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sciences, 9(2), 127–152. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00143739
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00143739...
; Slovic, 1987Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science, 236(4799), 280–285. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507...
; Slovic et al., 1982Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982). Why study risk perception? Risk Analysis, 2(2), 83–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1982.tb01369.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1982...
). Regarding health risk perception, the literature initially concerned deliberative risk perceptions based on rules about the likelihood of developing a disease (Denes-Raj & Epstein, 1994Denes-Raj, V., & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 819–829. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.819
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.8...
; Shafir et al., 1993Shafir, E., Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1993). Reason-based choice. Cognition, 49(1–2), 11–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90034-S
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)900...
). Recently, affective risk perception has been included, which corresponds to values (positive versus negative) and excitement (high versus low) associated with affective responses linked to the possibility of developing a disease or not, including worry or fear (Leventhal et al., 1980Leventhal, H., Meyer, D., & Nerenz, D. (1980). The common sense representation of illness danger. Contributions to Medical Psychology, 2, 7–30.).

It is noticeable that the perception of health risk can be enriched by analyzing multiple factors. Ferrer et al. (2016)Ferrer, R. A., Klein, W. M. P., Persoskie, A., Avishai-Yitshak, A., & Sheeran, P. (2016). The Tripartite Model of Risk Perception (TRIRISK): Distinguishing deliberative, affective, and experiential components of perceived risk. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 50(5), 653–663. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9790-z
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9790-...
ratified this question by checking deliberative, affective, and experiential factors. The latter are conceptualized as rule-based logical risk assessments, which correspond to the individual’s experiential processing, involving concrete images and holistic vision, among other aspects (Epstein et al., 1996Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390–405. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.3...
).

Gender differences

Studies indicate differences in risk perception between men and women (Flynn et al., 1994Flynn, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (1994). Gender, race, and perception of environmental health risks. Risk Analysis, 14(6), 1101–1108. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994.tb00082.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994...
; Weber et al., 2002Weber, E. U., Blais, A.-R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific riskattitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263–290. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414
https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414...
). Possible explanations for this phenomenon include social (Caroli & Weber-Baghdiguian, 2016Caroli, E., & Weber-Baghdiguian, L. (2016). Self-reported health and gender: The role of social norms. Social Science & Medicine, 153, 220–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.02.023
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016...
; Mahalik et al., 2015Mahalik, J. R., Lombardi, C. M., Sims, J., Coley, R. L., & Lynch, A. D. (2015). Gender, male-typicality, and social norms predicting adolescent alcohol intoxication and marijuana use. Social Science & Medicine, 143, 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.08.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015...
), biological (De Sio et al., 2017De Sio, S., Cedrone, F., Sanità, D., Ricci, P., Corbosiero, P., Di Traglia, M., Greco, E., & Stansfeld, S. (2017). Quality of life in workers and stress: Gender differences in exposure to psychosocial risks and perceived wellbeing. BioMed Research International, 7340781. https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/7340781
https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/7340781...
), cultural (Finucane et al., 2000Finucane, M. L., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Flynn, J., & Satterfield, T. A. (2000). Gender, race, and perceived risk: The “white male” effect. Health, Risk & Society, 2(2), 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1080/713670162
https://doi.org/10.1080/713670162...
), behavioral, and cognitive factors, such as risk preference, emotions (Garikipati & Kambhampati, 2021Garikipati, S., & Kambhampati, U. (2021). Leading the fight against the pandemic: Does gender really matter? Feminist Economics, 27(1–2), 401–418. https://doi.org/10.1080/13545701.2021.1874614
https://doi.org/10.1080/13545701.2021.18...
), and overconfidence (Croson & Gneezy, 2009Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448–474. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.47.2.448
https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.47.2.448...
). Women’s lack of knowledge and familiarity with science and technology (Morioka, 2014Morioka, R. (2014). Gender difference in the health risk perception of radiation from Fukushima in Japan: The role of hegemonic masculinity. Social Science and Medicine, 107, 105–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.014
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2014...
; Paul Slovic, 1999Slovic, P. (1999). Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. Risk Analysis, 19(4), 689–701. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1999.tb00439.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1999...
) has been cited as a relevant factor. Men take greater risks than women (Byrnes et al., 1999Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3....
; Harris et al., 2006Harris, C., Jenkins, M., & Glaser, D. (2006). Gender differences in risk assessment: Why do women take fewer risks than men? Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 48–63.). Health risks are no different, as in the United States and other countries, men are encouraged to take more health risks, such as high levels of alcohol consumption, a behavior considered a symbol of masculinity (Mahalik et al., 2015Mahalik, J. R., Lombardi, C. M., Sims, J., Coley, R. L., & Lynch, A. D. (2015). Gender, male-typicality, and social norms predicting adolescent alcohol intoxication and marijuana use. Social Science & Medicine, 143, 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.08.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015...
).

Women, in particular, are more sensitive to social norms regarding body weight issues, such as meeting the body mass index (BMI) and so-called beauty standards (Caroli & Weber-Baghdiguian, 2016Caroli, E., & Weber-Baghdiguian, L. (2016). Self-reported health and gender: The role of social norms. Social Science & Medicine, 153, 220–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.02.023
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016...
). Thus, they tend to report worse health than men (Barreto & Figueiredo, 2009Barreto, S. M., & Figueiredo, R. C. de (2009). Doença crônica, auto-avaliação de saúde e comportamento de risco: Diferença de gênero. Revista de Saúde Pública, 43(sup. 2), 38–47. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0034-89102009000900006
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0034-8910200900...
) and, consequently, become more able to perceive the effectiveness of drugs and the incidence of substance abuse and to believe in the effectiveness of prevention and treatment (Kauffman et al., 1997Kauffman, S. E., Silver, P., & Poulin, J. (1997). Gender differences in attitudes toward alcohol, tobacco, and other drugs. Social Work, 42(3), 231–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/42.3.231
https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/42.3.231...
). However, with the diagnosis of chronic diseases, men rated their health worse than women (Barreto & Figueiredo, 2009Barreto, S. M., & Figueiredo, R. C. de (2009). Doença crônica, auto-avaliação de saúde e comportamento de risco: Diferença de gênero. Revista de Saúde Pública, 43(sup. 2), 38–47. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0034-89102009000900006
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0034-8910200900...
).

Considering the context of the pandemic in terms of Covid-19 knowledge, risk perception, and precautionary behavior, adequate knowledge of Covid-19 was linked to higher involvement in precautionary behavior through risk perception for females but not for males (Abdelrahman, 2022Abdelrahman, M. (2022). Personality Traits, risk perception, and protective behaviors of Arab residents of Qatar during the Covid-19 pandemic. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 20(1), 237–248. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00352-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00352...
; Garikipati & Kambhampati, 2021Garikipati, S., & Kambhampati, U. (2021). Leading the fight against the pandemic: Does gender really matter? Feminist Economics, 27(1–2), 401–418. https://doi.org/10.1080/13545701.2021.1874614
https://doi.org/10.1080/13545701.2021.18...
; Iorfa et al., 2020Iorfa, S. K., Ottu, I. F. A., Oguntayo, R., Ayandele, O., Kolawole, S. O., Gandi, J. C., Dangiwa, A. L., & Olapegba, P. O. (2020). Covid-19 knowledge, risk perception, and precautionary behavior among Nigerians: A moderated mediation approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566773
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.56677...
; Schneider et al., 2021Schneider, C. R., Dryhurst, S., Kerr, J., Freeman, A. L. J., Recchia, G., Spiegelhalter, D., & Linden, S. van der (2021). Covid-19 risk perception: A longitudinal analysis of its predictors and associations with health protective behaviours in the United Kingdom. Journal of Risk Research, 24(3–4), 294–313. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2021.1890637
https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2021.18...
; Zhang et al., 2019Zhang, X., Wang, F., Zhu, C., & Wang, Z. (2019). Willingness to self-isolate when facing a pandemic risk: Model, empirical test, and policy recommendations. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(1), 197. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17010197
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17010197...
). This confirms that women are more sensitive to social norms based on risk perception. Evidence from psychology also suggests that women respond more strongly and intensely than men when anticipating negative outcomes, while men have been found to respond with anger to negative experiences. Therefore, if a negative outcome is anticipated as being worse by women than by men, women will be more risk-averse in situations like the current pandemic (Garikipati & Kambhampati, 2021Garikipati, S., & Kambhampati, U. (2021). Leading the fight against the pandemic: Does gender really matter? Feminist Economics, 27(1–2), 401–418. https://doi.org/10.1080/13545701.2021.1874614
https://doi.org/10.1080/13545701.2021.18...
; Rodriguez-Besteiro et al., 2021Rodriguez-Besteiro, S., Tornero-Aguilera, J. F., Fernández-Lucas, J., & Clemente-Suárez, V. J. (2021). Gender differences in the Covid-19 pandemic risk perception, psychology and behaviors of spanish university students. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(8), 3908. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083908
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083908...
) because, in some contexts, the less one knows about a hazard, the higher their risk perceptions (Jenkins et al., 2021Jenkins, S. C., Harris, A. J. L., & Osman, M. (2021). What drives risk perceptions? Revisiting public perceptions of food hazards associated with production and consumption. Journal of Risk Research, 24(11), 1450–1464. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1871057
https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.18...
).

As for risk awareness, there are gender differences in the perceived risk and knowledge of colorectal cancer. Women had less knowledge about this condition, presenting a mistaken sense of personal risk (McKinney & Palmer, 2014McKinney, S. Y., & Palmer, R. C. (2014). The influence of gender on colorectal cancer knowledge, screening intention, perceived risk and worry among African Americans in South Florida. Journal of Community Health, 39(2), 230–238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10900-013-9812-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10900-013-9812-...
). Gender was also associated with perceived risk awareness in tuberculosis, for which women had better results than men (Ailinger et al., 2003Ailinger, R. L., Lasus, H., & Dear, M. (2003). Americans’ knowledge and perceived risk of tuberculosis. Public Health Nursing, 20(3), 211–215. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0737-1209.2003.20308.x
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0737-1209.2003...
). Gender differences in health-related issues can also stem from demographic and socioeconomic conditions (Adler et al., 2016Adler, N. E., Glymour, M. M., & Fielding, J. (2016). Addressing social determinants of health and health inequalities. JAMA, 316(16), 1641–1642. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2016.14058
https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2016.14058...
; Patrão & McIntyre, 2017Patrão, A. L., & McIntyre, T. M. (2017). Socio-demographic, marital, and psychosocial factors associated with condom use negotiation self-efficacy among Mozambican women at risk for HIV infection. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 24(6), 846–855. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-017-9681-0
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-017-9681-...
; Pylypchuk & Kirby, 2017Pylypchuk, Y., & Kirby, J. B. (2017). The role of marriage in explaining racial and ethnic disparities in access to health care for men in the US. Review of Economics of the Household, 15(3), 807–832. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11150-015-9300-2
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11150-015-9300-...
). Relationships between poverty, psychosocial stress, and obesity (Kwarteng et al., 2017Kwarteng, J. L., Schulz, A. J., Mentz, G. B., Israel, B. A., & Perkins, D. W. (2017). Independent effects of neighborhood poverty and psychosocial stress on obesity over time. Journal of Urban Health, 94(6), 791–802. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11524-017-0193-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11524-017-0193-...
) confirm the influence of socioeconomic elements on health (Cundiff et al., 2016Cundiff, J. M., Kamarck, T. W., & Manuck, S. B. (2016). Daily interpersonal experience partially explains the association between social rank and physical health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 50(6), 854–861. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9811-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9811-...
). In households where men are providers, the allocation of health expenditures can be based on the preferences of the “head of household,” which can lead to less access to health services by women (Onah & Horton, 2018Onah, M. N., & Horton, S. (2018). Male-female differences in households’ resource allocation and decision to seek healthcare in south-eastern Nigeria: Results from a mixed methods study. Social Science & Medicine, 204, 84–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.03.033
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018...
). In families where adolescents perceive high levels of parental monitoring, male adolescents consume less tobacco, and women, fewer drugs and less alcohol (Nelson et al., 2017Nelson, K. M., Carey, K. B., Scott-Sheldon, L. A. J., Eckert, T. L., Park, A., Vanable, P. A., Ewart, C. K., & Carey, M. P. (2017). Gender differences in relations among perceived family characteristics and risky health behaviors in urban adolescents. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 51(3), 416–422. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9865-x
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9865-...
).

Personality traits also contribute to the explanation of gender differences. By analyzing the five personality domains, Beyer et al. (2015)Beyer, A. R., Fasolo, B., De Graeff, P. A., & Hillege, H. L. (2015). Risk attitudes and personality traits predict perceptions of benefits and risks for medicinal products: A field study of European medical assessors. Value in Health, 18(1), 91–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2014.10.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2014.10.0...
concluded that an increase in the conscientiousness score increased the perceived benefit of a medication, considering men’s scores are higher than women’s. However, some studies indicate that for some medications and treatments, no differences are accorded to gender observation. In analyzing syndromes such as hypochondria and depression (Gureje et al., 1997Gureje, O., Üstün, T. B., & Simon, G. E. (1997). The syndrome of hypochondriasis: A cross-national study in primary care. Psychological Medicine, 27(5), 1001–1010. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291797005345
https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329179700534...
; Piccinelli & Wilkinson, 2000Piccinelli, M., & Wilkinson, G. (2000). Gender differences in depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177(6), 486–492. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.177.6.486
https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.177.6.486...
) and in treatment for lung cancer (MacLean et al., 2017MacLean, A., Hunt, K., Smith, S., & Wyke, S. (2017). Does gender matter? An analysis of men’s and women’s accounts of responding to symptoms of lung cancer. Social Science & Medicine, 191, 134–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.09.015
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017...
), men and women took similar initiatives. In summary, gender differences in risk perception can be considered from different perspectives: 1. they can perceive different levels of the same risk, 2. they can perceive different risks, or 3. they can assign different meanings to the same risk (Gustafson, 1998Gustafson, P. E. (1998). Gender differences in risk perception: Theoretical and methodological perspectives. Risk Analysis, 18(6), 805–811. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:rian.0000005926.03250.c0
https://doi.org/10.1023/b:rian.000000592...
).

DATA AND METHODS

Data collection and variables

The dataset comprised 1,191 instruments applied at random in the largest city in South America, São Paulo, Brazil. The questionnaire considered the perception of the risks and benefits of medications and medical and nonmedical procedures. Both the questionnaire and the data are available in Mendes-Da-Silva (2022)Mendes-Da-Silva, W. (2022). Data to replicate “perceived risks and benefits in medical remedies and procedures: what do men and women think?”, published by Revista de Administração Mackenzie, Mendeley Data, V2. https://doi.org/10.17632/z8zptfwj7n.2
https://doi.org/10.17632/z8zptfwj7n.2...
. From an operational view, the measurement of perceived risk followed the psychometric and quantitative paradigm. Through the psychological paradigm, this study followed the psychometric model developed by Slovic et al. (1989)Slovic, P., Kraus, N., Lappe, H., Letzel, H., & Malmfors, T. (1989). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Report on a survey in Sweden. In B. Horisberger, R. Dinkel (Eds.), The perception and management of drug safety risks. Springer. and which is already used in studies in Sweden, Canada, and the United States (Slovic et al., 1989Slovic, P., Kraus, N., Lappe, H., Letzel, H., & Malmfors, T. (1989). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Report on a survey in Sweden. In B. Horisberger, R. Dinkel (Eds.), The perception and management of drug safety risks. Springer., 1991Slovic, P., Kraus, N., Lappe, H., & Major, M. (1991). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Report on a survey in Canada. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 82(3), S15–S20., 2007Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
). The following dimensions assessed perceived risk: risk alertness, risk awareness, and risk of personal injury.

For the evaluation, 52 items were chosen and separated into three categories (medicines: 31 items; medical procedures: eight items; and non-medical procedures: 13 items) according to several criteria, such as importance, familiarity to the general public, and diversity (Slovic et al., 2007Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
). We proposed the following question to analyze the risk of alertness dimension: “If you hear or read about a particular problem or incident associated with each item in which people were seriously harmed, at what level would that episode serve as an alert, indicating that the risk of each item could be higher than you thought before this episode occurred?”. To measure the perception of risk of personal injury, we used the following question: “To what extent would you say that people exposed to these items are at risk of personal injury?”.

The risk awareness dimension was observed while considering this question: “To what extent would you say that those exposed to these risks precisely know the risks associated with these items?”. To evaluate the perceived benefits, we asked: “In general, how beneficial is the item?”. Only the minimum and maximum were defined to determine the scale, providing free interpretation. Table 1 synthesizes the instrument.

Table 1
Information on the data collection instrument

The analysis strategy encompassed three phases: descriptive, bivariate, and extended regression model (ERM). To make comparisons by gender (men and women), the chi-square test and the Student’s t-test were used. The estimation via ERM allows the observation of endogenous variables, that is, those correlated with the model’s error term, so two other common problems can also be considered, like non-random selection and assignment of exogenous and endogenous treatment (Drukker, 2017Drukker, D. (2017). Estimating effects from extended regression models. In 2017 London Stata Users Group meeting. Cass Business School, London.). For the estimation, medications’ benefits were the dependent variable, and the risk dimensions and dummy variables were the independent variables. The influence of risk perception on perceived benefit was analyzed with two models. In model (1), the ERM was estimated with the endogeny evaluation, whereas in model (2), besides the endogeny, gender was an exogenous treatment variable. Equations 1, 2, and 3 represent the estimated model.

(1) P B i = α i + i = 1 3 β i R i s k i + i = 4 12 β i D Y i + β i E n d o g M P i + β i E n d o g N M I i + ε i

(2) E n d o g M P i ( B e n e f M P i = α i + β 1 P I M P i + β 2 R K M P i + β 3 R A M P i + ε i )

(3) E n d o g N M I i ( B e n e f N M I i = α i + β 1 P I N M I i + β 2 R K N M I i + β 3 R A N M I i + ε i )

in which: PBi is the perceived benefits of medications; Riski = the risk dimension variables (risk alertness, risk awareness, and risk of personal injury); DYi = the dummy variables, which are constructed as follows: gender (0 = men, 1 = women), works in the industry (0 = no, 1 = yes), awareness of prescription’ problems (0 = no, 1 = yes), live with the elderly or sick (0 = no, 1 = yes), take care of the elderly or infirm (0 = no, 1 = yes), use medication regularly (0 = no, 1 = yes), have dysfunctions (0 = no, 1 = yes), health insurance (0 = no, 1 = yes), side effect (0 = no, 1 = yes); εi = error term; EndogMPi and EndogNMIi are the endogenous variables, which were estimated considering the benefits based on the three risk dimensions; NMI = non- -medical items; MP = medical procedures; PI = risk of personal injury; RK = risk awareness; RA = risk alertness. Table 2 summarizes the models, variables, and main references.

Table 2
Models, equations, variables, and main references

For the estimation of the models, the main research hypotheses were considered:

  • H1: The lower the perceptions of personal injury and the higher the perceptions of risk alertness and risk awareness, the greater the perceived benefits.

  • H2: Women perceive greater benefits of medications than men.

Respondents’ profile

Of the 1,191 respondents, 52% were women, 44% had only completed high school, and 40% had a degree. The average age was 37 (minimum 16 and maximum 97, standard deviation 14.7). As for occupation, 13.7% worked in the healthcare industry, and 58.6% reported being aware of recent problems with prescription drugs or medical errors. One-third of respondents lived with elderly people or people with serious health problems, and 24.1% were actively involved in caring for sick, elderly, or highly inactive people. Regarding tobacco use, it was found that 38.0% had consumed cigarettes, 17.3% hookahs, and 6.6% cigars. However, approximately one-third (32.3%) reported never using any tobacco in the past. Consumption is heterogeneous, with 13.8% smoking daily or almost daily, and another 17.7% claiming to smoke only one to three times a month. Still, 32.7% declared they smoked only a few cigarettes a day, and 34.4% smoked a pack or more. Among those who had quit smoking, 47% had quit for over two years. Among the 12 disorders, high cholesterol (28.0%), high blood pressure (17.4%), and diabetes (12.8%) stand out. Medication usage was significant among respondents, with 47.9% consuming one, 22.0% two, and 9.8% three medications regularly. Table 3 presents the most consumed medications in the last five years.

Table 3
Medication consumption per gender

The most relevant differences are medications for allergies, laxatives, anxiety, hormone replacement, osteoporosis, and erectile dysfunction. Antibiotics and vaccines were the most commonly used medications, with no differences being found according to gender (p > 0.1), corroborating Slovic et al. (2007)Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
. The abuse of antibiotics is common in all regions of the world (Gourgoulis et al., 2013Gourgoulis, G. M., Katerelos, P., Maragos, A., Gargalianos, P., Lazanas, M., & Maltezou, H. C. (2013). Antibiotic prescription and knowledge about antibiotic costs of physicians in primary health care centers in Greece. American Journal of Infection Control, 41(12), 1296–1297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2013.02.016
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2013.02.0...
), so in Brazil, the government has conditioned their sale on the presentation of special control prescriptions. As for vaccines, Brazil has an aggressive public policy aimed at reducing diseases using intensive campaigns for childhood vaccination (Ministry of Health, 2003). This policy aligns with efforts to increase access to vaccines in developing countries, such as the Global Alliance for Vaccines (Dickens, 2011Dickens, T. (2011). The world medicines situation 2011: procurement of medicines. In The world medicines situation report 2011. World Health Organization. https://www.cabdirect.org/cabdirect/abstract/20113168482
https://www.cabdirect.org/cabdirect/abst...
).

RESULTS

About 34% of men and women considered drugs to work as expected. For 17.4% of men and 20.3% of women, patients often experienced side effects. As patients, 38% of respondents said they had suffered side effects from medication in the last five years. Of these patients, 35.1% considered the effect mild, 35.7% moderate, and 29.2% severe. The most frequent causes of side effects were related to noncompliance with prescribed instructions (in both genders) and the patient’s sensitivity to the drug, especially in women, with a percentage of 29.8% in options 5 and 6, corroborating Slovic et al. (2007)Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
.

Bivariate analysis

The risk dimensions and perceived benefits of medication consumption (Figure 1), medical procedures, and non-medical items were verified for both genders. It can be observed that women saw a greater benefit from some medications (biotechnology, anti-inflammatory, cholesterol, diet, allergy, Viagra, osteoporosis, arthritis, HRT, and blood pressure). Men saw equal or fewer benefits than women, with no higher benefits. As for the lowest perceived benefits, medications for these situations were cited: depression, Aids, sleeping, nicotine, and chemotherapy. Risk perception was higher in the risk alertness dimension, in which responses in both genders ranged from levels 3 to 6. In the personal injury dimension, vitamin pills had a low risk of personal injury, which follows findings in the United States, Canada, and Sweden (Slovic et al., 2007Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
).

Figure 1
Perception of benefits and risks of medications according to gender

Risk awareness is sometimes lower, and personal injury and risk alertness assessment is relevant. This is the case with diet pills, which may be linked to poor knowledge of side effects, leading to a personal alert. This difference is more salient in women, who demonstrated higher levels of the perceived risk of personal injury and risk alertness. This corroborates women’s greater concern about food safety and issues involving body weight (Caroli & Weber-Baghdiguian, 2016Caroli, E., & Weber-Baghdiguian, L. (2016). Self-reported health and gender: The role of social norms. Social Science & Medicine, 153, 220–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.02.023
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016...
). Regarding gender, differences such as the perception of personal injury in antibiotics can be highlighted. The medians are distinct, showing that women perceive a higher consumption risk, yet they claim to use them more often than men (Figure 1). Higher antibiotic use by women can be explained by more common infections in this gender, such as urinary tract infections (Shaifali et al., 2012Shaifali, I., Gupta, U., Mahmood, S. E., & Ahmed, J. (2012). Antibiotic susceptibility patterns of urinary pathogens in female outpatients. North American Journal of Medical Sciences, 4(4), 163–169. https://doi.org/10.4103/1947-2714.94940
https://doi.org/10.4103/1947-2714.94940...
).

Regarding risk awareness, the differences are in medications for osteoporosis and vitamin pills (men with higher perception) and chemotherapy (women with higher perception). In risk alertness, women demonstrated higher levels related to the medications for anxiety (Fallon et al., 2012Fallon, B. A., Harper, K. M., Landa, A., Pavlicova, M., Schneier, F. R., Carson, A., Harding, K., Keegan, K., Schwartz, T., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2012). Personality disorders in hypochondriasis: Prevalence and comparison with two anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics, 53(6), 566–574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psym.2012.02.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psym.2012.02.0...
), contraception, sleep, diet, and Botox (Beyer et al., 2013Beyer, A. R., Fasolo, B., Phillips, L. D., De Graeff, P. A., & Hillege, H. L. (2013). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a survey among experts in the European regulatory network. Medical Decision Making, 33(4), 579–592. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X12472397
https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X12472397...
). The same analysis was performed for medical procedures. Risk alertness retained the highest perception in most procedures, with results similar to those of Slovic et al. (2007)Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
. In the analysis by gender, there was a balance between the medians, especially in the risk awareness dimension, showing that the perceptions are similar.

Comparing by categories, the dimensions of risk in non-medical elements were evaluated, with the perception of risk of personal injury found to be higher for smoking (Hoover et al., 2018Hoover, D. S., Wetter, D. W., Vidrine, D. J., Nguyen, N., Frank, S. G., Li, Y., Waters, A. J., Meade, C. D., & Vidrine, J. I. (2018). Enhancing Smoking risk communications: The influence of health literacy and message content. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 52(3), 204–215. https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kax042
https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kax042...
) and nuclear power plants (Slovic et al., 2007Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
). Nuclear power plants can cause anxiety because living near these sites can pose high-magnitude risks to human health (Lyons et al., 2020Lyons, B. A., Akin, H., & Stroud, N. J. (2020). Proximity (mis) perception: Public awareness of nuclear, refinery, and fracking sites. Risk Analysis, 40(2), 385–398. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13387
https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13387...
). Then, the respondents’ perception of the benefits of medications and their relationship with the risks was pointed out (Figure 2). The respondent’s average risk (the average risk of the three dimensions) was considered for this calculation.

Figure 2
Perceived benefits and risks of the consumption of medications and nonmedical items

Among medications, benefits ranged from 3 to 5, which shows a reasonable perception of benefits. The greatest perception of benefits was found in vaccines for both men and women. The lowest perception of benefits matched the results of Slovic et al. (2007)Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
, corresponding to Botox injection. Men and women had similar perceptions of the risk-benefit relationship, especially for Viagra, for which the results were nearly equivalent. For women, the highest risk was attributed to chemotherapy, which may be linked to women’s lower risk awareness regarding cancer (McKinney & Palmer, 2014McKinney, S. Y., & Palmer, R. C. (2014). The influence of gender on colorectal cancer knowledge, screening intention, perceived risk and worry among African Americans in South Florida. Journal of Community Health, 39(2), 230–238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10900-013-9812-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10900-013-9812-...
). For men, the greatest risk was attributed to depression medications.

In the risk-benefit relationship, six medications (antidepressants, sleeping pills, anxiety medication, Botox injections, diet pills, and erectile dysfunction medication) had a negative relationship. Respondents had a perception that these medications pose higher risks. In medical procedures, superior relationships were found for prostate exams (Farrel et al., 2002Farrell, M., Murphy, M., & Schneider, C. (2002). How underlying patient beliefs can affect physician-patient communication about prostate-specific antigen testing. Effective Clinical Practice, 5, 120–129.; Slovic et al., 2007Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
), indicating that the benefits of this procedure outweigh the associated risks, especially in women’s view. The highest risk level for both genders was associated with cardiac surgeries (Beyer et al., 2013Beyer, A. R., Fasolo, B., Phillips, L. D., De Graeff, P. A., & Hillege, H. L. (2013). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a survey among experts in the European regulatory network. Medical Decision Making, 33(4), 579–592. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X12472397
https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X12472397...
).

In the non-medical items category, the highest benefits were found for automobiles and air travel, possibly due to the transportation optimization they provided. Yet, in the benefit-risk relationship, there were four items (smoking, high-fat foods, nuclear power plants, alcoholic beverages) with a negative relation. The respondents indicated a higher average risk than the offered benefit (Hoover et al., 2018Hoover, D. S., Wetter, D. W., Vidrine, D. J., Nguyen, N., Frank, S. G., Li, Y., Waters, A. J., Meade, C. D., & Vidrine, J. I. (2018). Enhancing Smoking risk communications: The influence of health literacy and message content. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 52(3), 204–215. https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kax042
https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kax042...
; Slovic et al., 2007Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
). Afterward, differences were observed in the means between men and women in risk aspects, including personal injury, risk awareness, risk alertness, and benefits (Table 4).

Table 4
Evaluation of risk and benefit by gender and product type

In the analysis of benefits in the medication category, there were differences in means for ten products. Respondents assigned greater benefits to vaccines, and women perceived a greater benefit. Significant differences in means for prostate exams and acupuncture are shown for medical procedures. In both, women had higher means of perceived benefits. The lower benefits men assign to prostate exams may be related to beliefs, such as some analogies and even fear of cancer (Farrel et al., 2002Farrell, M., Murphy, M., & Schneider, C. (2002). How underlying patient beliefs can affect physician-patient communication about prostate-specific antigen testing. Effective Clinical Practice, 5, 120–129.). Confirming this result, in the analysis of the risk of personal injury, men obtained higher means, i.e., they perceived greater personal injury risk than women did. For non- -medical risks, the significant difference corresponds to alcoholic beverages, for which women were more aware of the risk and had a greater perception of risk alertness. Men’s lower risk perception of alcohol consumption corroborates Flynn et al. (1994)Flynn, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (1994). Gender, race, and perception of environmental health risks. Risk Analysis, 14(6), 1101–1108. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994.tb00082.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994...
, Kauffman et al. (1997)Kauffman, S. E., Silver, P., & Poulin, J. (1997). Gender differences in attitudes toward alcohol, tobacco, and other drugs. Social Work, 42(3), 231–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/42.3.231
https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/42.3.231...
, and Maričić et al. (2013)Maričić, J., Sučić, I., & Šakić, V. (2013). Risk perception related to (il)licit substance use and attitudes towards its’ use and legalization: The role of age, gender and substance use. Drustvena Istrazivanja, 22(4), 579–599. https://doi.org/10.5559/di.22.4.02
https://doi.org/10.5559/di.22.4.02...
.

In the dimension of personal injury, mean differences are seen in several items. For medications, the highest means of personal injury were indicated for sleeping pills and the lowest for aspirin. In both cases, women had a higher perception of the risk of personal injury. The study by Slovic et al. (2007)Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
showed a greater perception of the risk of personal injury for nuclear power plants by women, a result also evidenced by the research in Brazil. Flynn et al. (1994)Flynn, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (1994). Gender, race, and perception of environmental health risks. Risk Analysis, 14(6), 1101–1108. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994.tb00082.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994...
pointed out that women had higher risk perceptions of technological dangers than men. In the dimension of risk awareness, differences are evident in three cases, of which the highest mean represents Alzheimer’s medications, of which men claimed to have greater risk awareness than women.

In risk alertness, women had higher risk perceptions from diet and arthritis medications and medical procedures for chemotherapy and tomography. Regarding chemotherapy, this result may be related to women’s higher perception of cancer risk (Taber et al., 2017Taber, J. M., Klein, W. M. P., Suls, J. M., & Ferrer, R. A. (2017). Lay Awareness of the relationship between age and cancer risk. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 51(2), 214–225. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9845-1
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9845-...
). In the benefits and risk assessment (personal injury and alertness), women presented higher means than men, results that may be related to the higher general risk perception of this gender (Flynn et al., 1994Flynn, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (1994). Gender, race, and perception of environmental health risks. Risk Analysis, 14(6), 1101–1108. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994.tb00082.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994...
). Women are also more likely to believe in the prevention and effectiveness of treatments, perceiving medications as powerful products that, therefore, have higher perceived benefits (Kauffman et al., 1997Kauffman, S. E., Silver, P., & Poulin, J. (1997). Gender differences in attitudes toward alcohol, tobacco, and other drugs. Social Work, 42(3), 231–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/42.3.231
https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/42.3.231...
). The risk awareness category for medications is an exception, as for the three medications with a significant difference, men’s perception was higher. Behavior in the alcoholic beverages category stands out, as it is the only product with significant differences in all dimensions analyzed. Women’s perception of benefits was significantly lower than men’s, and their perception of all risks was significantly higher. This result corroborates the incentive men receive to consume alcoholic beverages (Mahalik et al., 2015Mahalik, J. R., Lombardi, C. M., Sims, J., Coley, R. L., & Lynch, A. D. (2015). Gender, male-typicality, and social norms predicting adolescent alcohol intoxication and marijuana use. Social Science & Medicine, 143, 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.08.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015...
).

Regressions

The ERM was used to estimate the regressions. The model-building considered that the perceived benefit of using medications is influenced by the perception of the dimensions of the risk of medications (risk alertness, personal injury, and risk awareness), gender, and experience in healthcare and care of the elderly and sick (Table 5).

Table 5
Models for determining medication benefits

Model 1 shows that the perception of the benefits of medications can be explained by the dimensions of the risk of medications (personal injury, risk alertness, and risk awareness), gender, regular medication usage, and the benefits of medical procedures. Regarding risk dimensions, the lower the perceptions of personal injury and the higher the perceptions of risk alertness and risk awareness, the greater the perceived benefits of medication usage, confirming hypothesis 1. Regular use of medications also contributed to broadening the perception of benefits. The three risk dimensions and acts influenced the risk perception of medical procedures as an endogenous variable positively related to the benefits of medications. Individuals who saw greater benefits from medical procedures also tended to see greater benefits in medications. On the other hand, no evidence was obtained that the perception of benefits relative to the category of non-medical items influenced medication benefits.

Considering the significant effect of gender in model 1, indicating that women perceived greater benefits of medications (Beyer et al., 2013Beyer, A. R., Fasolo, B., Phillips, L. D., De Graeff, P. A., & Hillege, H. L. (2013). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a survey among experts in the European regulatory network. Medical Decision Making, 33(4), 579–592. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X12472397
https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X12472397...
), model 2 was estimated (gender as an exogenous treatment variable). It is observed that the perceptions of injury and risk alertness and the benefits of procedures contributed to women’s perception of medication benefits. The results support hypothesis 2. In the group of men, besides these three significant variables for women, risk awareness is evidenced as positively related to the benefit of medications. For the variables related to experience in healthcare and care for the elderly and sick and for side effects, the results did not show gender significance, as in model 1. In summary, the marginal effect of gender on medication benefits is approximately nine percentage points for women compared to men. For both, the risks of personal injury decrease, and the risk alerts increase the perception of benefits. Only in the group of men does risk awareness become relevant to the perceived benefits.

DISCUSSION

In the group of medications, it was observed that for six items, the average risk perception outweighed the perception of benefits: antidepressants, anxiety medication, sleeping pills, Botox, diet pills, and erectile dysfunction medication. Among these, there is low consumption by society, with sleeping pills being consumed by 11.6% and erectile dysfunction medication by only 1.5% of respondents. However, medications for blood pressure, asthma, and Alzheimer’s have a benefit/risk relationship higher than 1, i.e., their perceived benefits outweigh their perceived risks by more than one point on the scale. In this group, the high level of benefits, low perception of personal injury, and reasonable perception of risk alert assigned to vaccines by both men and women also stand out. Considering several governments use a vaccination schedule, especially for children, the population apparently understands the benefits of joining the campaigns. However, they remain on risk alert, possibly due to commonly established adverse reactions.

The results for non-medical procedures show that all evaluated items had a positive benefit/risk relationship, especially the prostate exam, with the largest difference, and the IUD, with the smallest. For the prostate exam, women attributed more benefits and less risk of personal injury than men did. These results seem to explain men’s “resistance” to this test. The nonmedical risks category was established for comparison with other categories that directly involved health-related aspects. It is noteworthy that, in this category, for all items except alcoholic beverages, men and women had similar perceptions of benefits, but women perceived greater risks of personal injury. Thus, while both genders perceived similar benefits and risk awareness levels for actions such as smoking, driving, flying, and eating genetically modified or high-fat foods, women viewed higher risks of personal injury.

In this category, alcoholic beverages are the only item for which women’s perception of benefits was significantly lower than that of males, and their perception of all risks was significantly higher. The fact that men perceive more benefits and fewer risks from drinking alcohol is related to its being a symbol of masculinity, as highlighted in the literature. Another highlight is the risk of smoking, which obtained the highest average risk perception and the worst average benefit in the men’s and women’s groups. This demonstrates that campaigns on the consequences of smoking, such as those printed on cigarette packs and those carried out by government media, and the creation of laws to prevent smoking in public places, such as companies, bars, and restaurants, among others, contribute to the public’s awareness of the benefits and risks involved in smoking.

In the analysis by gender, it was observed that women presented superior benefit perceptions but similar risk perceptions for several medications. For medications for which differences in risk perception were observed, such as diet pills, aspirin, and laxatives, and for procedures such as IUD implantation and non-medical risks like drinking alcoholic beverages, women had higher perceptions than men, consistent with results from developed countries. Women had a higher risk perception in personal injury and risk alertness than men. By inquiring more about medicines and seeking help with health issues, women realize less risk awareness, greater risk alertness, and personal injury. Because of overconfidence, men think they know the risks better and underestimate the perception of alertness and injuries. The analysis by items indicates there are differences between emerging and developed countries. In Slovic et al.’s (1991Slovic, P., Kraus, N., Lappe, H., & Major, M. (1991). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Report on a survey in Canada. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 82(3), S15–S20., 1989Slovic, P., Kraus, N., Lappe, H., Letzel, H., & Malmfors, T. (1989). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Report on a survey in Sweden. In B. Horisberger, R. Dinkel (Eds.), The perception and management of drug safety risks. Springer., 2007Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0092861507041001...
) studies, men had higher risk perceptions than women for items such as medications for depression, blood pressure, Aids, osteoporosis, and ulcers, which were not found in this study. Still, vaccines are the medications with the highest perceived benefits, a distinct result from developed countries, where medications such as insulin and antibiotics have the greatest perceived benefits.

The results are representative of Brazil. However, they cannot be considered a standard for emerging countries. In other words, geographic, cultural, political, and other aspects can influence the perception of risks and benefits. It would be interesting to explore further how risk and benefit perceptions are formed, especially considering the emergence of new drugs and vaccines, as seen in the Covid-19 pandemic. Public policies and healthcare professionals should ensure that patients are well informed about the medicines they take, as how patients perceive the risks of drug prescriptions can influence treatment preferences and drug-taking decisions (Hughes et al., 2002Hughes, L., Whittlesea, C., & Luscombe, D. (2002). Patients’ knowledge and perceptions of the side-effects of OTC medication. Journal of Clinical Pharmacy and Therapeutics, 27(4), 243–248. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2710.2002.00416.x
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2710.2002...
). Providing clear and understandable information about diseases and treatments, including risks and side effects, can have beneficial effects on patient satisfaction and bring important health outcomes (Riva et al., 2012Riva, S., Monti, M., Iannello, P., & Antonietti, A. (2012). The representation of risk in routine medical experience: What Actions for contemporary health policy? PLoS One, 7(11), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048297
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.004...
), as empirical evidence has shown that the likelihood rating for infectious disease (affecting humans) was impacted by gender (Brown et al., 2021Brown , G. D., Largey, A., & McMullan, C. (2021). The impact of gender on risk perception: Implications for EU member states’ national risk assessment processes. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction, 63, 102452. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102452
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102...
).

Health policy is about giving information and how to present it, at what time, and how it will be interpreted. Studies in cognitive psychology and health prevention have shown that information must go beyond traditional reports, so presenting information in different formats can help people think about the risks and benefits more actively and deliberately (Riva et al., 2012Riva, S., Monti, M., Iannello, P., & Antonietti, A. (2012). The representation of risk in routine medical experience: What Actions for contemporary health policy? PLoS One, 7(11), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048297
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.004...
). Using new technologies, such as cell phones, has the potential to reduce medication errors, ensure replenishment, and digitally verify patient records, helping to minimize risks (WHO, 2009).

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The authors would like to thank professors Paul Slovic, Ellen Peters, John Grana, Susan Berger, and Gretchen S. Dieck for their kindness in providing the original questionnaire adapted for this research. We thank the Brazilian Institute of Financial Innovation (Ibrif) and Bells & Bayes Rating Analytics for the financial support that allowed this study to be carried out. Finally, we thank each individual who voluntarily answered the questions in the questionnaire. In return to society, we have decided to publish this article duly accompanied by the collection instrument and the collected data, which are publicly available at: https://doi.org/10.17632/z8zptfwj7n.2

REFERENCES

  • Abdelrahman, M. (2022). Personality Traits, risk perception, and protective behaviors of Arab residents of Qatar during the Covid-19 pandemic. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 20(1), 237–248. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00352-7
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00352-7
  • Abrams, E. M., & Greenhawt, M. (2020). Risk communication during Covid-19. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology: In Practice, 8(6), 1791–1794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaip.2020.04.012
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaip.2020.04.012
  • Adler, N. E., Glymour, M. M., & Fielding, J. (2016). Addressing social determinants of health and health inequalities. JAMA, 316(16), 1641–1642. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2016.14058
    » https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2016.14058
  • Ailinger, R. L., Lasus, H., & Dear, M. (2003). Americans’ knowledge and perceived risk of tuberculosis. Public Health Nursing, 20(3), 211–215. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0737-1209.2003.20308.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0737-1209.2003.20308.x
  • Axon, R. N., Bradford, W. D., & Egan, B. M. (2009). The role of individual time preferences in health behaviors among hypertensive adults: A pilot study. Journal of the American Society of Hypertension, 3(1), 35–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jash.2008.08.005
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jash.2008.08.005
  • Baer, W. (2014). The Brazilian economy: Growth & development Lynne Rienner.
  • Barreto, S. M., & Figueiredo, R. C. de (2009). Doença crônica, auto-avaliação de saúde e comportamento de risco: Diferença de gênero. Revista de Saúde Pública, 43(sup. 2), 38–47. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0034-89102009000900006
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/s0034-89102009000900006
  • Beck, U. (1992). Risk society: Towards a new modernity University of Munich.
  • Betz, N. E., Weber, E. U., & Blais, A.-R. (2002). A domain-specific riskattitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263–290. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414
  • Beyer, A. R., Fasolo, B., De Graeff, P. A., & Hillege, H. L. (2015). Risk attitudes and personality traits predict perceptions of benefits and risks for medicinal products: A field study of European medical assessors. Value in Health, 18(1), 91–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2014.10.011
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2014.10.011
  • Beyer, A. R., Fasolo, B., Phillips, L. D., De Graeff, P. A., & Hillege, H. L. (2013). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a survey among experts in the European regulatory network. Medical Decision Making, 33(4), 579–592. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X12472397
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X12472397
  • Bodemer, N., & Gaissmaier, W. (2015). Risk perception. In H. Cho, T. Reimer, & K. McComas (Eds.), The Sage handbook of risk communication Sage.
  • Bolton, J. M., Robinson, J., & Sareen, J. (2009). Self-medication of mood disorders with alcohol and drugs in the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Affective Disorders, 115(3), 367–375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.003
  • Bradford, W. D. (2010). The association between individual time preferences and health maintenance habits. Medical Decision Making, 30(1), 99–112. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X09342276
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X09342276
  • Brandt, S., & Dickinson, B. (2013). Time and risk preferences and the use of asthma controller medication. Pediatrics, 131(4), e1204–e1210. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2982
    » https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2982
  • Breuer, W., Kollath, J., Salzmann, A., & Nitzsch, R. (2017). The assessment of risk behavior: A cross-cultural analysis using the DOSPERT scale. In I. Venezia (Eds.), Behavioral finance: Where do investors’ biases come from? World Scientific Publishing
  • Brown , G. D., Largey, A., & McMullan, C. (2021). The impact of gender on risk perception: Implications for EU member states’ national risk assessment processes. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction, 63, 102452. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102452
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102452
  • Burgess, A. (2015). Social construction of risk. In H. Cho, T. Reimer, K. McComas. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of risk communication Sage.
  • Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367
  • Caroli, E., & Weber-Baghdiguian, L. (2016). Self-reported health and gender: The role of social norms. Social Science & Medicine, 153, 220–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.02.023
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.02.023
  • Chaplin, T. M., Hong, K., Bergquist, K., & Sinha, R. (2008). Gender differences in response to emotional stress: An assessment across subjective, behavioral, and physiological domains and relations to alcohol craving. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32(7), 1242–1250. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00679.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00679.x
  • Chapman, G. B., & Coups, E. J. (1999). Predictors of influenza vaccine acceptance among healthy adults. Preventive Medicine, 29(4), 249–262. https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1999.0535
    » https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1999.0535
  • Chapple, C. L., & Johnson, K. A. (2007). Gender differences in impulsivity. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 5(3), 221–234. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541204007301286
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1541204007301286
  • Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448–474. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.47.2.448
    » https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.47.2.448
  • Cuevas, A., Karpowicz, I., Granados, C., & Soto, M. (2017). Fiscal challenges of population aging in Brazil. IMF Working Papers, 17(99). https://doi.org/10.5089/9781475595550.001
    » https://doi.org/10.5089/9781475595550.001
  • Cundiff, J. M., Kamarck, T. W., & Manuck, S. B. (2016). Daily interpersonal experience partially explains the association between social rank and physical health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 50(6), 854–861. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9811-y
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9811-y
  • De Sio, S., Cedrone, F., Sanità, D., Ricci, P., Corbosiero, P., Di Traglia, M., Greco, E., & Stansfeld, S. (2017). Quality of life in workers and stress: Gender differences in exposure to psychosocial risks and perceived wellbeing. BioMed Research International, 7340781. https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/7340781
    » https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/7340781
  • Denes-Raj, V., & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 819–829. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.819
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.819
  • Dickens, T. (2011). The world medicines situation 2011: procurement of medicines. In The world medicines situation report 2011. World Health Organization. https://www.cabdirect.org/cabdirect/abstract/20113168482
    » https://www.cabdirect.org/cabdirect/abstract/20113168482
  • Drukker, D. (2017). Estimating effects from extended regression models. In 2017 London Stata Users Group meeting. Cass Business School, London.
  • Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390–405. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390
  • Fallon, B. A., Harper, K. M., Landa, A., Pavlicova, M., Schneier, F. R., Carson, A., Harding, K., Keegan, K., Schwartz, T., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2012). Personality disorders in hypochondriasis: Prevalence and comparison with two anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics, 53(6), 566–574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psym.2012.02.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psym.2012.02.002
  • Farrell, M., Murphy, M., & Schneider, C. (2002). How underlying patient beliefs can affect physician-patient communication about prostate-specific antigen testing. Effective Clinical Practice, 5, 120–129.
  • Ferrer, R. A., Klein, W. M. P., Persoskie, A., Avishai-Yitshak, A., & Sheeran, P. (2016). The Tripartite Model of Risk Perception (TRIRISK): Distinguishing deliberative, affective, and experiential components of perceived risk. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 50(5), 653–663. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9790-z
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9790-z
  • Filia, S. L., Baker, A. L., Gurvich, C. T., Richmond, R., & Kulkarni, J. (2014). The perceived risks and benefits of quitting in smokers diagnosed with severe mental illness participating in a smoking cessation intervention: Gender differences and comparison to smokers without mental illness. Drug and Alcohol Review, 33(1), 78–85. https://doi.org/10.1111/dar.12091
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/dar.12091
  • Finucane, M. L., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Flynn, J., & Satterfield, T. A. (2000). Gender, race, and perceived risk: The “white male” effect. Health, Risk & Society, 2(2), 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1080/713670162
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/713670162
  • Fischhoff, B. (1983). “Acceptable risk”: The case of nuclear power. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 2(4), 559–575. https://doi.org/10.2307/3323574
    » https://doi.org/10.2307/3323574
  • Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., Lichtenstein, S., Read, S., & Combs, B. (1978). How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sciences, 9(2), 127–152. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00143739
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00143739
  • Flynn, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (1994). Gender, race, and perception of environmental health risks. Risk Analysis, 14(6), 1101–1108. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994.tb00082.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994.tb00082.x
  • Galizzi, M. M., Miraldo, M., Stavropoulou, C., & Pol, M. van der (2016). Doctor–patient differences in risk and time preferences: A field experiment. Journal of Health Economics, 50, 171–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2016.10.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2016.10.001
  • Gardner, G. E., & Jones, M. G. (2011). Science instructors’ perceptions of the risks of biotechnology: Implications for science education. Research in Science Education, 41(5), 711–738. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-010-9187-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-010-9187-0
  • Garikipati, S., & Kambhampati, U. (2021). Leading the fight against the pandemic: Does gender really matter? Feminist Economics, 27(1–2), 401–418. https://doi.org/10.1080/13545701.2021.1874614
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/13545701.2021.1874614
  • Gourgoulis, G. M., Katerelos, P., Maragos, A., Gargalianos, P., Lazanas, M., & Maltezou, H. C. (2013). Antibiotic prescription and knowledge about antibiotic costs of physicians in primary health care centers in Greece. American Journal of Infection Control, 41(12), 1296–1297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2013.02.016
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2013.02.016
  • Green, C. (2006). Gender and use of substance abuse treatment services. Alcohol Research and Health, 29(1), 55–62.
  • Greenfield, S. F., Back, S. E., Lawson, K., & Brady, K. T. (2010). Substance abuse in women. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33(2), 339–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2010.01.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2010.01.004
  • Grigoryan, L., Haaijer-Ruskamp, F. M., Burgerhof, J. G. M., Mechtler, R., Deschepper, R., Tambic-Andrasevic, A., Andrajati, R., Monnet, D. L., Cunney, R., Di Matteo, A., Edelstein, H., Valinteliene, R., Alkerwi, A., Scicluna, E. A., Grzesiowski, P., Bara, A. C., Tesar, T., Cizman, M., Campos, J. … Birkin, J. (2006). Self-medication with antimicrobial drugs in Europe. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 12(3), 452–459. https://doi.org/10.3201/eid1203.050992
    » https://doi.org/10.3201/eid1203.050992
  • Gureje, O., Üstün, T. B., & Simon, G. E. (1997). The syndrome of hypochondriasis: A cross-national study in primary care. Psychological Medicine, 27(5), 1001–1010. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291797005345
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291797005345
  • Gustafson, P. E. (1998). Gender differences in risk perception: Theoretical and methodological perspectives. Risk Analysis, 18(6), 805–811. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:rian.0000005926.03250.c0
    » https://doi.org/10.1023/b:rian.0000005926.03250.c0
  • Hansen, J., & Hammann, M. (2017). Risk in science instruction: The realist and constructivist paradigms of risk. Science and Education, 26(7–9), 749–775. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-017-9923-1
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-017-9923-1
  • Hardon, A., Hodgkin, C., & Fresle, D. (2004). How to investigate the use of medicines by consumers World Health Organization, University of Amsterdam.
  • Harris, C., Jenkins, M., & Glaser, D. (2006). Gender differences in risk assessment: Why do women take fewer risks than men? Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 48–63.
  • Hernandez, J., & Kellner, R. (1992). Hypochondriacal concerns and attitudes toward illness in males and females. The International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 22(3), 251–263. https://doi.org/10.2190/W9KG-6HU9-5QJX-NW76
    » https://doi.org/10.2190/W9KG-6HU9-5QJX-NW76
  • Hoover, D. S., Wetter, D. W., Vidrine, D. J., Nguyen, N., Frank, S. G., Li, Y., Waters, A. J., Meade, C. D., & Vidrine, J. I. (2018). Enhancing Smoking risk communications: The influence of health literacy and message content. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 52(3), 204–215. https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kax042
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kax042
  • Hopkin, P. (2017). Fundamentals of risk management: Understanding, evaluating and implementing effective risk management Kogan Page.
  • Hughes, L., Whittlesea, C., & Luscombe, D. (2002). Patients’ knowledge and perceptions of the side-effects of OTC medication. Journal of Clinical Pharmacy and Therapeutics, 27(4), 243–248. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2710.2002.00416.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2710.2002.00416.x
  • Hunt, K., Adamson, J., Hewitt, C., & Nazareth, I. (2011). Do women consult more than men? A review of gender and consultation for back pain and headache. Journal of Health Services Research and Policy, 16(2), 108–117. https://doi.org/10.1258/jhsrp.2010.009131
    » https://doi.org/10.1258/jhsrp.2010.009131
  • Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística (2017). Estimativas da população residente nos municípios brasileiros com base em 1º de julho de 2017.
  • Iorfa, S. K., Ottu, I. F. A., Oguntayo, R., Ayandele, O., Kolawole, S. O., Gandi, J. C., Dangiwa, A. L., & Olapegba, P. O. (2020). Covid-19 knowledge, risk perception, and precautionary behavior among Nigerians: A moderated mediation approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566773
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566773
  • Jenkins, S. C., Harris, A. J. L., & Osman, M. (2021). What drives risk perceptions? Revisiting public perceptions of food hazards associated with production and consumption. Journal of Risk Research, 24(11), 1450–1464. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1871057
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1871057
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 430–454. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(72)90016-3
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(72)90016-3
  • Kandall, S. R. (2010). Women and drug addiction: A historical perspective. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 29(2), 117–126. https://doi.org/10.1080/10550881003684491
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10550881003684491
  • Kaplan, W., & Mathers, C. (2011). Global health trends: Global burden of disease and pharmaceutical needs. In World Health Organization (Ed.), The world medicines situation 2011
  • Kauffman, S. E., Silver, P., & Poulin, J. (1997). Gender differences in attitudes toward alcohol, tobacco, and other drugs. Social Work, 42(3), 231–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/42.3.231
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/42.3.231
  • Krewski, D., Lemyre, L., Turner, M. C., Lee, J. E. C., Dallaire, C., Bouchard, L., Brand, K., Mercier, P. (2006). Public perception of population health risks in Canada: Health hazards and sources of information. Human and Ecological Risk Assessment, 12(4), 626–644. https://doi.org/10.1080/10807030600561832
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10807030600561832
  • Kwarteng, J. L., Schulz, A. J., Mentz, G. B., Israel, B. A., & Perkins, D. W. (2017). Independent effects of neighborhood poverty and psychosocial stress on obesity over time. Journal of Urban Health, 94(6), 791–802. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11524-017-0193-7
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11524-017-0193-7
  • Letzel, H. (1989). Statistics in drug risk research: The background of pharmacoepidemiology. In B. Horisberger, R. Dinkel (Eds.), The perception and management of drug safety risks Springer.
  • Leventhal, H., Meyer, D., & Nerenz, D. (1980). The common sense representation of illness danger. Contributions to Medical Psychology, 2, 7–30.
  • Levy, I., Cohen-Louck, K., & Bonny-Noach, H. (2021). Gender, employment, and continuous pandemic as predictors of alcohol and drug consumption during the Covid-19. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 228, 109029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.109029
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.109029
  • London, A. S., & Robles, A. (2000). The co-occurrence of correct and incorrect HIV transmission knowledge and perceived risk for HIV among women of childbearing age in El Salvador. Social Science & Medicine, 51(8), 1267–1278. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00044-7
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00044-7
  • Lucas, E. C., Mendes-Da-Silva, W., & Lyons, A. C. (2017). Gender differences in attitudes towards driving and demand for private Insurance: Evidence from middle class drivers. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 47, 72–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2017.04.022
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2017.04.022
  • Lyons, B. A., Akin, H., & Stroud, N. J. (2020). Proximity (mis) perception: Public awareness of nuclear, refinery, and fracking sites. Risk Analysis, 40(2), 385–398. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13387
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13387
  • MacLean, A., Hunt, K., Smith, S., & Wyke, S. (2017). Does gender matter? An analysis of men’s and women’s accounts of responding to symptoms of lung cancer. Social Science & Medicine, 191, 134–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.09.015
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.09.015
  • Mahalik, J. R., Lombardi, C. M., Sims, J., Coley, R. L., & Lynch, A. D. (2015). Gender, male-typicality, and social norms predicting adolescent alcohol intoxication and marijuana use. Social Science & Medicine, 143, 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.08.013
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.08.013
  • Maričić, J., Sučić, I., & Šakić, V. (2013). Risk perception related to (il)licit substance use and attitudes towards its’ use and legalization: The role of age, gender and substance use. Drustvena Istrazivanja, 22(4), 579–599. https://doi.org/10.5559/di.22.4.02
    » https://doi.org/10.5559/di.22.4.02
  • McKinney, S. Y., & Palmer, R. C. (2014). The influence of gender on colorectal cancer knowledge, screening intention, perceived risk and worry among African Americans in South Florida. Journal of Community Health, 39(2), 230–238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10900-013-9812-8
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10900-013-9812-8
  • Mendes-Da-Silva, W. (2022). Data to replicate “perceived risks and benefits in medical remedies and procedures: what do men and women think?”, published by Revista de Administração Mackenzie, Mendeley Data, V2. https://doi.org/10.17632/z8zptfwj7n.2
    » https://doi.org/10.17632/z8zptfwj7n.2
  • Ministério da Saúde (2003). Programa Nacional de Imunizações: 30 anos
  • Morioka, R. (2014). Gender difference in the health risk perception of radiation from Fukushima in Japan: The role of hegemonic masculinity. Social Science and Medicine, 107, 105–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.014
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.014
  • Nelson, K. M., Carey, K. B., Scott-Sheldon, L. A. J., Eckert, T. L., Park, A., Vanable, P. A., Ewart, C. K., & Carey, M. P. (2017). Gender differences in relations among perceived family characteristics and risky health behaviors in urban adolescents. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 51(3), 416–422. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9865-x
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9865-x
  • Nguyen, K. V., Do, N. T. T., Chandna, A., Nguyen, T. V., Pham, C. V., Doan, P. M., Nguyen, A. Q., Nguyen, C. K. T., Larsson, M., Escalante, S., Olowokure, B., Laxminarayan, R., Gelband, H., Horby, P., Ngo, H. B. T., Hoang, M. T., Farrar, J., Hien, T. T., & Wertheim, H. F. (2013). Antibiotic use and resistance in emerging economies: A situation analysis for Viet Nam. BMC Public Health, 13(1), 1158. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-1158
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-1158
  • O’Connor, S. S., Comtois, K. A., Atkins, D. C., & Kerbrat, A. H. (2017). Examining the impact of suicide attempt function and perceived effectiveness in predicting reattempt for emergency medicine patients. Behavior Therapy, 48(1), 45–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.05.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.05.004
  • Olsen, R. A. (2001). Behavioral finance as science: Implications from the research of Paul Slovic. Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets, 2(3), 157–159. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327760JPFM0203_5
    » https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327760JPFM0203_5
  • Onah, M. N., & Horton, S. (2018). Male-female differences in households’ resource allocation and decision to seek healthcare in south-eastern Nigeria: Results from a mixed methods study. Social Science & Medicine, 204, 84–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.03.033
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.03.033
  • Patrão, A. L., & McIntyre, T. M. (2017). Socio-demographic, marital, and psychosocial factors associated with condom use negotiation self-efficacy among Mozambican women at risk for HIV infection. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 24(6), 846–855. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-017-9681-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-017-9681-0
  • Paudel, S., Palaian, S., Shankar, P. R., & Subedi, N. (2021). Risk perception and hesitancy toward Covid-19 vaccination among healthcare workers and staff at a medical college in Nepal. Risk Management and Healthcare Policy, 14, 2253–2261. https://doi.org/10.2147/RMHP.S310289
    » https://doi.org/10.2147/RMHP.S310289
  • Piccinelli, M., & Wilkinson, G. (2000). Gender differences in depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177(6), 486–492. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.177.6.486
    » https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.177.6.486
  • Picone, G., Sloan, F., & Taylor, D. (2004). Effects of risk and time preference and expected longevity on demand for medical tests. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 28(1), 39–53. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:RISK.0000009435.11390.23
    » https://doi.org/10.1023/B:RISK.0000009435.11390.23
  • Pylypchuk, Y., & Kirby, J. B. (2017). The role of marriage in explaining racial and ethnic disparities in access to health care for men in the US. Review of Economics of the Household, 15(3), 807–832. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11150-015-9300-2
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11150-015-9300-2
  • Renn, O. (2008). White paper on risk governance: Toward an integrative framework. In O. Renn, K. Walker (Eds.), Global risk governance: Concept and practice using the IRGC framework Springer.
  • Riva, S., Monti, M., Iannello, P., & Antonietti, A. (2012). The representation of risk in routine medical experience: What Actions for contemporary health policy? PLoS One, 7(11), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048297
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048297
  • Rodriguez-Besteiro, S., Tornero-Aguilera, J. F., Fernández-Lucas, J., & Clemente-Suárez, V. J. (2021). Gender differences in the Covid-19 pandemic risk perception, psychology and behaviors of spanish university students. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(8), 3908. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083908
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083908
  • Schneider, C. R., Dryhurst, S., Kerr, J., Freeman, A. L. J., Recchia, G., Spiegelhalter, D., & Linden, S. van der (2021). Covid-19 risk perception: A longitudinal analysis of its predictors and associations with health protective behaviours in the United Kingdom. Journal of Risk Research, 24(3–4), 294–313. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2021.1890637
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2021.1890637
  • Shafir, E., Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1993). Reason-based choice. Cognition, 49(1–2), 11–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90034-S
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90034-S
  • Shaifali, I., Gupta, U., Mahmood, S. E., & Ahmed, J. (2012). Antibiotic susceptibility patterns of urinary pathogens in female outpatients. North American Journal of Medical Sciences, 4(4), 163–169. https://doi.org/10.4103/1947-2714.94940
    » https://doi.org/10.4103/1947-2714.94940
  • Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99–118. https://doi.org/10.2307/1884852
    » https://doi.org/10.2307/1884852
  • Sitkin, S. B., & Pablo, A. L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the determinants of risk behavior. Academy of Management Review, 17(1), 9–38. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1992.4279564
    » https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1992.4279564
  • Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science, 236(4799), 280–285. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507
    » https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507
  • Slovic, P. (1999). Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. Risk Analysis, 19(4), 689–701. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1999.tb00439.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1999.tb00439.x
  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24(2), 311–322. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x
  • Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982). Why study risk perception? Risk Analysis, 2(2), 83–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1982.tb01369.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1982.tb01369.x
  • Slovic, P., Kraus, N., Lappe, H., Letzel, H., & Malmfors, T. (1989). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Report on a survey in Sweden. In B. Horisberger, R. Dinkel (Eds.), The perception and management of drug safety risks Springer.
  • Slovic, P., Kraus, N., Lappe, H., & Major, M. (1991). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Report on a survey in Canada. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 82(3), S15–S20.
  • Slovic, P, Peters, E., Grana, J., Berger, S., & Dieck, G. S. (2007). Risk perception of prescription drugs: Results of a national survey. Drug Information Journal, 41(1), 81–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/009286150704100110
  • Taber, J. M., Klein, W. M. P., Suls, J. M., & Ferrer, R. A. (2017). Lay Awareness of the relationship between age and cancer risk. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 51(2), 214–225. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9845-1
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9845-1
  • Vieira, E. M., & Ford, N. J. (1996). The provision of female sterilization in São Paulo, Brazil: A study among low income women. Social Science and Medicine, 42(10), 1427–1432. https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(95)00291-X
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(95)00291-X
  • Weber, E. U., Blais, A.-R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific riskattitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263–290. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414
  • World Health Organization (2009). Continuity and change: Implementing the third WHO Medicines Strategy 2008-2013
  • Zhang, X., Wang, F., Zhu, C., & Wang, Z. (2019). Willingness to self-isolate when facing a pandemic risk: Model, empirical test, and policy recommendations. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(1), 197. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17010197
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17010197

Edited by

EDITORIAL BOARD
Editor-in-chief
Fellipe Silva Martins
Associated editor
Janaina Macke
Technical support
Gabriel Henrique Carille
EDITORIAL PRODUCTION
Publishing coordination
Jéssica Dametta
Language editor
Irina Migliari (Bardo Editorial)
Layout designer
Emap
Graphic designer
Libro

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    15 Dec 2023
  • Date of issue
    2024

History

  • Received
    04 Feb 2021
  • Accepted
    24 Aug 2022
Editora Mackenzie; Universidade Presbiteriana Mackenzie Rua da Consolação, 896, Edifício Rev. Modesto Carvalhosa, Térreo - Coordenação da RAM, Consolação - São Paulo - SP - Brasil - cep 01302-907 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista.adm@mackenzie.br