Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The cherology awareness in Portuguese sign language 1 1 Responsible Editor: Alexandre Filordi de Carvalho. - https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4510-9440 2 2 References correction and bibliographic normalization services: Vera Lúcia Fator Gouvêa Bonilha – verah.bonilha@gmail.com. 3 3 English version: Claudia Bentes – traducao@tikinet.com.br

Abstract

The Portuguese Sign Language (Linguagem Gestual Portuguesa, LGP) is a structured and natural language and should therefore be elected to enable the learning process of Portuguese deaf children. However, there is still a need to expand its knowledge, especially about the various acquisition periods. Special attention should be paid to cherology - study of the appropriation and the process of construction of this competence by the child/subject. The scarce evidence on cherology awareness demonstrate that its development plays an important role in the process of language acquisition. Therefore, the children environment must be supportive and promote contact with their natural language. Due to the relevance of linguistic environment, it is important to address the different contexts in the acquisition of sign language, as well as the cherology parameters. Finally, the conceptual and methodological gaps will be addressed. There is no existent measure for the assessment of cherology awareness.

Keywords
language acquisition; acquisition contexts; deaf; Portuguese sign language; cherology awarenes; assessment

Resumo

A língua gestual portuguesa (LGP) é uma língua estruturada e natural, devendo, por isso, ser a escolhida no processo de ensino-aprendizagem das crianças surdas portuguesas. No entanto, há a necessidade de expandir o seu conhecimento, sobretudo sobre as etapas de desenvolvimento e aquisição, dando especial atenção à consciência querológica ‒ estudo da apropriação e o processo de construção desta competência pela criança/sujeito. Apesar da necessidade de maior aprofundamento dessa temática, as evidências apontam que o seu desenvolvimento assume um papel importante no processo de aquisição da linguagem. Neste sentido, atendendo à escassez de informação científica sobre a temática, este artigo, em formato de revisão de literatura, propõe-se a abordar a aquisição da LGP, através da descrição detalhada dos seus estágios, bem como dos diferentes contextos de aquisição. A consciência querológica é, então, abordada no âmbito da sua aquisição, parâmetros e lacunas ‒ conceituais e metodológicas ‒, inerentes ao seu estudo e avaliação.

Palavras-chave
aquisição da linguagem; contextos de aquisição; surdo; língua gestual portuguesa; querologia; avaliação

Acquisition and development of Portuguese Sign Language

The theories on language acquisition and development are vast and diverse. According to Chomsky (2006)Chomsky, N. (2006). Language and mind. Cambridge: University Press. ISBN: 9780521674935., the predisposition to acquire language is an innate ability of the human being. In the course of these innate-based theories, Lenneberg (1967)Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language. With Appendices by Noam Chomksy and Otto Marx. Wiley. points to the Critical Period Hypothesis, an ideal window for language acquisition, with its peak around age 2, a consequence of the brain neuroplasticity evidenced by children in the process of language acquisition (Neville & Bavelier, 2002Neville, H., & Bavelier, D. (2002). Human brain plasticity: evidence from sensory deprivation and altered language experience. Progress in Brain Research, 138, 177-188. Doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(02)38078-6.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(02)38...
). Some authors corroborate the first idea, arguing that the neuronal circuits responsible for language acquisition are regulated by the experiences lived during the critical period (Johnson & Newport, 1989Johnson, S., & Newport, E. (1989). Critical periods effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21(1), 60-99. Doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(89)900...
; Knudsen, 2004Knudsen, E. (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1412-1425. Doi: 10.1162/0898929042304796.
https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042304796...
). Chomsky’s theory (Chomsky, 1959Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B.F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Language, 35(I), 26-58.), in contrast to the behaviorist idea (Augusto, 1995Augusto, M. (1995). Teoria gerativa e aquisição da linguagem. Sitientibus, 13, 115-120. http://www2.uefs.br/sitientibus/pdf/13/teoria_gerativa_e_aquisicao_da_linguagem.pdf.
http://www2.uefs.br/sitientibus/pdf/13/t...
) - individuals are clean slates, with no natural ability to acquire language, which happens only from social interaction - assumes that human beings have a genetic predisposition concerning an innate ability to produce, understand, recognize and acquire language.

Thus, language is defined as an innate ability (Chomsky, 2006Chomsky, N. (2006). Language and mind. Cambridge: University Press. ISBN: 9780521674935.) that humans have to produce, understand, and develop language, and that serves as a tool for them to communicate and interact with each other (Franco et al. 2003Franco, M., Reis, M., & Gil, S. (2003). Domínio da comunicação, linguagem e fala. Perturbações específicas de linguagem em contexto escolar: fundamentos. Ministério da Educação.). Its acquisition and development result from the (quality of) interaction between the child and the environment (Sim-Sim, 1998Sim-Sim, I. (1998). Desenvolvimento da linguagem. Universidade Aberta.), which, the more stimulating it is, and the richer the experiences lived, the greater the chances of developing the linguistic component of their natural language (Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.; Sim-Sim et al., 2008Sim-Sim, I., Silva, A., & Nunes, C. (2008). Linguagem e Comunicação no jardim de infância: textos de apoio para educadores de infância. Ministério da Educação, Direção - Geral de Inovação e Desenvolvimento Curricular.).

The mother tongue, spoken in the family and with emotional weight, is naturally acquired and conditions the socialization process. The second language, on the other hand, is a language learned in a school context (or not), and is not natural to the child’s family circle (Spinassé, 2006Spinassé, P. (2006). Os conceitos língua materna, segunda língua e língua estrangeira e os falantes de línguas alóctones minoritárias no Sul do Brasil. Revista Contingentia, 1, 1-10.). It is therefore legitimate that deaf children, like hearing children, have access to their own natural language (Rubio & Queiroz, 2014Rubio, S., & Queiroz, S. (2014). A aquisição da linguagem e integração social: a libras como formadora de identidade do surdo. Revista Eletrônica Saberes da Educação, 5(1), 1-15.), a language (input and output) visual-manual or sign language (Batista, 2010Batista, M. (2010). Alunos surdos: aquisição da língua gestual e ensino da Língua Portuguesa. Exedra, 9, 1-12.; Mineiro et al., 2008Mineiro, A., Duarte, L., Carvalho, P., Tebé, C., & Correia, M. (2008). Aspectos da polissemia nominal em língua gestual portuguesa. Revista Polissema, 8, 37-56.), or Portuguese sign language (LGP), which will ground the learning of a second language, i.e., Portuguese (Batista, 2010Batista, M. (2010). Alunos surdos: aquisição da língua gestual e ensino da Língua Portuguesa. Exedra, 9, 1-12.). Deaf children need to internalize the linguistic rules of the LGP, going through the same phases and stages of acquisition of oral language by hearing children (Newport, 1988Newport, E. (1988). Constraints on learning and their role in language acquisition: studies on the acquisition of american sign language. Language Science, 10(1), 147-172. Doi: 10.1016/0388-0001(88)90010-1.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0388-0001(88)900...
; Petitto, 1987Petitto, L. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27(1), 1-52. Doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90034-5;
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)900...
; Petitto & Marentette, 1991Petitto, A., & Marantette, P. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251, 1483 - 1496.). Sign language processing is performed in the same place (left hemisphere) as speech and sound, i.e., oral language (Pettito & Marentette, 1991Petitto, A., & Marantette, P. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251, 1483 - 1496.; Pettito et al., 2000Petitto, L., Zatorre, R., Gauna, K., Nikelski, E., Dostie, D., & Evns, A. (2000). Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: implications for the neural basis of human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of SSciences of the United States of America, 97(25), 13961-13966. Doi: 10.1073./pnas.97.25.13961.
https://doi.org/10.1073./pnas.97.25.1396...
; Quadros et al., 2010Quadros, R., Cruz, R., & Pizzio, A. (2010). Desenvolvimento da língua de sinais: a determinação do input. Trabalho apresentado no 8º congresso internacional da ISAPL (Society of applied psycholinguistics). Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul.), and the human brain is programmed to build any language matrix. Although exposure to oral linguistic input is more available to deaf children, since about 90% are children of hearing parents (Batista, 2010Batista, M. (2010). Alunos surdos: aquisição da língua gestual e ensino da Língua Portuguesa. Exedra, 9, 1-12.), oral language was not created in order to meet the biological needs of deaf children, since it does not present a sensory modality available and possible for them, often leading to poor and limited language acquisition.

Deaf children of deaf parents who are exposed to their natural and mother tongue, the Sign Language (Lillo-Martin, 1986Lillo-Martin, D. (1986). Parameter setting: evidence from use, acquisition, and breakdown in American Sign Language. [Tese de Doutorado]. University of California, San Diego. University Microfilms International. Ann Arbor. Michigan.; Meier, 1987Meier, R. (1987). Elicited imitation of verb agreement in american sign language: iconically or morphologically determined? Journal of Memory and Language, 6(3), 362-376. Doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90119-7.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)901...
; Petitto, 1987Petitto, L. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27(1), 1-52. Doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90034-5;
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)900...
), develop grammatical rules in a similar way to hearing children in relation to oral language (Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.) in a facilitated communicative process (Reis, 1997Reis, V. (1997). A linguagem e seus efeitos no desenvolvimento cognitivo e emocional da criança surda. Espaço Informativo Técnico Científico do INES, 6, 23-38.). On the other hand, the acquisition of sign language proficiency by deaf children with hearing parents who do not master sign language will be compromised (Neville & Bavelier, 2002Neville, H., & Bavelier, D. (2002). Human brain plasticity: evidence from sensory deprivation and altered language experience. Progress in Brain Research, 138, 177-188. Doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(02)38078-6.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(02)38...
; Newport, 1990Newport, E. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11-28. Doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2;
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1401...
; Singleton & Newport, 1994Singleton, J., & Newport, E. (1994). When learners surpass their models: the acquisition of american sign language from inconsistent input. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 370-407. Doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.05.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004....
) at several levels, including cultural and individual identity (Amaral, 1999Amaral, I. (1999). Comunicação e linguagem. In F. Pereira (Cord.), O aluno surdo em contexto escolar. Apoios educativos (pp. 37-48). Ministério da Educação.). According to Neves and Miranda (s.d.), when deaf children are not exposed to sign language early on, they will create their own gestures, with their own grammatical rules, seeming to contribute to the natural impulse that human beings have in language acquisition (Chomsky, 1959Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B.F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Language, 35(I), 26-58.). The language acquisition of Portuguese deaf children should be made through their natural language, i.e., LGP, expressed through a visual-spatial modality (Quadros, 1997Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem. Artmed.), being the only one that allows them to access a linguistic and communicative input for a perfect development of their language (Amaral, 2006Amaral, A. (2006). Perspetivas teóricas na aquisição da linguagem em crianças surdas. In M. Bispo, A. Couto, M. Clara, & L. Clara (org.), O Gesto e a Palavra I: Antologia de textos sobre a surdez (pp. 109-150). Lisboa: Caminho. ISBN: 9789722117913.).

The second context to which the child is exposed after the family, will be the school. In the Portuguese case, they are expected to attend the Reference Schools for Bilingual Education of Deaf Students (Escolas de Referência para Educação Bilingue de Alunos Surdos, EREBAS), which guarantee “a bilingual education that ensures the full cognitive and communicative development of the deaf child” (Batista et al. 2011Batista, J., Santiago, A., Almeida, D., Antunes, P., & Gaspar, R. (2011). Programa de Português L2 para alunos surdos. Ministério da Educação, Direção-Geral de Inovação e Desenvolvimento Curricular., p.7, free translation). It is in these schools that children acquire the language and live with their deaf peers, from preschool education to high school (Lima, 2011Lima, R. (2011). Fonologia infantil: aquisição, avaliação e intervenção. Almedina. ISBN: 9789724037837.). According to Carmo et al. (2007)Carmo, H., Martins, M., Morgado, M., & Estanqueiro, P. (2007). Programa curricular de língua gestual portuguesa educação pré-escolar e ensino básico. Ministério da Educação, Direcção-Geral de Inovação e de Desenvolvimento Curricular., bilingual education is now seen not as a necessity for deaf students, but as a right (Decree-Law No. 54/2018Diário da República. (2018, 6 jul.). Decreto-Lei n.º 54/2018. Regime Jurídico da Educação Inclusiva (1.ª série, n. 129, pp. 2918-2928).) aiming at the competence in both languages: the natural language (LGP) and the official language of the country (Portuguese) (Carmo et al., 2007Carmo, H., Martins, M., Morgado, M., & Estanqueiro, P. (2007). Programa curricular de língua gestual portuguesa educação pré-escolar e ensino básico. Ministério da Educação, Direcção-Geral de Inovação e de Desenvolvimento Curricular.).

When deaf children express their first gestures, they usually do so with modifications in the cheremes, i.e., in their parameters, changing configurations or producing inappropriate movements, corresponding to the execution of simpler gestures, and the semantic fields more easily grasped are those related to the child’s environment (family, food, animals, toys, means of transport, clothing, etc.) (Carmo et al., 2007Carmo, H., Martins, M., Morgado, M., & Estanqueiro, P. (2007). Programa curricular de língua gestual portuguesa educação pré-escolar e ensino básico. Ministério da Educação, Direcção-Geral de Inovação e de Desenvolvimento Curricular.). Caregivers are the first to detect differences in development when compared to peers, highlighting the importance of the age at which they realize that the child is deaf. The average age at which a deaf child is diagnosed is around 18 and a half months (Geffner et al.1978Geffner, D., Harry L., Freeman L., & Gaffney R. (1978). Speech and language assessment scales of deaf children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 11(2-3), 215-226. Doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(78)90014-X.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(78)900...
). The whole process from diagnosis to realization may take about 2 years.

Quadros et al. (2010)Quadros, R., Cruz, R., & Pizzio, A. (2010). Desenvolvimento da língua de sinais: a determinação do input. Trabalho apresentado no 8º congresso internacional da ISAPL (Society of applied psycholinguistics). Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul. evaluated the language development of deaf children and adolescents with different ages of onset of exposure to sign language (Brazilian sign language), or input, also considering the context of acquisition. The sample was divided into two different groups: those who acquired sign language before the age of 4/6, and those who acquired between the ages of 7 and 9. The analysis of the length of exposure to language is crucial. Children who acquired sign language before the age of 4/6 years show a more consistent vocabulary development. All those evaluated (100%) who had early language acquisition are able to correctly establish the use of classifiers in the language. On the other hand, children with early exposure, but less than 4 years of exposure, show some difficulties in establishing referents in space.

The deaf child, born and raised in a hearing family, develops coping strategies, heavily depending on their mother figure, with a tendency to oral language (Reis, 1997Reis, V. (1997). A linguagem e seus efeitos no desenvolvimento cognitivo e emocional da criança surda. Espaço Informativo Técnico Científico do INES, 6, 23-38.), damaging the maturation process that induces autonomy (Lane et al. 1996Lane, H., Hoffmeister, R., & Bahan, B. (1996). A journey into the deaf world. Dawn Sign Publishers.). Thus, it is crucial that deaf child comes into contact with their natural language early on, through early intervention. They should often socialize with fluent sign language persons that may, in some way, assist in the process of acquisition and maturation of sign language. It is also up to the parents of these children to encourage the study of this language, so that within the family, sign language may be used as the main fashion of communication.

Singleton and Newport (1994)Singleton, J., & Newport, E. (1994). When learners surpass their models: the acquisition of american sign language from inconsistent input. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 370-407. Doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.05.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004....
found that deaf children exposed to sign language after the age of 12, when compared to those exposed from birth, showed greater difficulties in constructing sentences. Meier (2002)Meier, P. (2002). The acquisition of verb agreement: pointing out arguments for the linguistic status of agreement in signed languages. Directions in sign language acquisition, 115-141. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.2.08mei.
https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.2.08mei...
also mentions that the ability to verbal agreement, as well as other morphological aspects, significantly depends on the critical period of acquisition. If there is no exposure to sign language, their language may be limited to the use of isolated and familiar gestures, serving only to express basic needs (Amaral & Coutinho, 2002Amaral, A., & Coutinho, A. (2002). A criança surda: educação e inserção social. Análise Psicológica, 3, 373-378. doi: 10.14417/ap.324.
https://doi.org/10.14417/ap.324...
).

LGP Stages

One should consider that, from an early age, deaf child communicates not only by looking and babbling, but also by gesture and facial expression, being able to distinguish gestures, as the hearing child can distinguish words (Schirmer et al. 2004Schirmer, C., Fontoura, D., & Nunes, M. (2004). Distúrbios da aquisição da linguagem e da aprendizagem. Jornal de Pediatria. Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria 80(2), 95-103. Doi: 10.1590/S0021-75572004000300012.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0021-7557200400...
). 2004Schirmer, C., Fontoura, D., & Nunes, M. (2004). Distúrbios da aquisição da linguagem e da aprendizagem. Jornal de Pediatria. Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria 80(2), 95-103. Doi: 10.1590/S0021-75572004000300012.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0021-7557200400...
). These authors highlight two main stages: 1) pre-linguistic, which consists of phonetic production, which in sign languages means making small meaningless configurations (from birth to 11 months); and 2) linguistic, which involves the production of small gestures with isolated and/or combined meanings, until they begin to expressing themselves through more complex structures, between the ages of 1 and 14 (Costa et al. 2002Costa, D., Azambuja, L., & Nunes, M. (2002). Avaliação do desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor. In M. Nunes, & A. Marrone (Eds), Semiologia Neurologica. (pp. 351-360), Porto Alegre.; Schirmer et al., 2004Schirmer, C., Fontoura, D., & Nunes, M. (2004). Distúrbios da aquisição da linguagem e da aprendizagem. Jornal de Pediatria. Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria 80(2), 95-103. Doi: 10.1590/S0021-75572004000300012.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0021-7557200400...
). This acquisition assumes visual discrimination, body control of movements, rules governing social interaction, understanding and expression of communicative intentions, based on the discovery of the surrounding universe (Carmo et al., 2007Carmo, H., Martins, M., Morgado, M., & Estanqueiro, P. (2007). Programa curricular de língua gestual portuguesa educação pré-escolar e ensino básico. Ministério da Educação, Direcção-Geral de Inovação e de Desenvolvimento Curricular.).

The first stage - pre-linguistic - involves organizing the neurophysiological and psychological structures of the child (Lima, 2000Lima, R. (2000). Linguagem infantil. Da normalidade à patologia. APPCDM Distrital de Braga.) with babbling, which evolves from reflex to an approximation of the phonetic models of the mother tongue, in all children (hearing and deaf), whose production is expressed by sound (syllabic babbling with combinations that are part of the phonetic system of sign languages), and by gestural means (e. g.: raising your arms to ask for your lap or point to something you want (Fernandes, 2003Fernandes, E. (2003). linguagem e surdez. Artmed Editora.; Petitto & Marantette, 1991Petitto, A., & Marantette, P. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251, 1483 - 1496.; Quadros, 1997Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem. Artmed.). Deaf children produce gestural syllabic sequences equivalent to the syllabic units found in vocal babbling (Petitto & Marantette, 1991Petitto, A., & Marantette, P. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251, 1483 - 1496.). Differences can be found in the behavior of deaf and hearing infants (Sim-Sim, 1998Sim-Sim, I. (1998). Desenvolvimento da linguagem. Universidade Aberta.) with the development of their modality babbling. Between 12 months and 2 years, the stage of a gesture takes place (Quadros, 1997Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem. Artmed.), disappearing the recourse to “pointing” which is replaced by grammatical elements of the language (Petitto, 1987Petitto, L. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27(1), 1-52. Doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90034-5;
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)900...
) or by gestures with broader meaning (Ingram, 1982). Just as an example: the recourse to the GO FOR A WALK gesture is used to convey “I want to go for a walk” or “Mom went for a walk” in association with the child’s daily activity, such as: MILK, MOTHER, FATHER (Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.), among others.

Although, in general, children produce isolated words or gestures to talk about things in their daily life, it is in the stage of early combinations (from 2 years old on), due to the increase in the child’s lexical field, that the first gestural combinations appear (Pettito & Marentette, 1991; Quadros, 1997Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem. Artmed.), with production supported by the combination of two or more gestures (Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.), and in a certain order. The premature sentence structure used by deaf children in this stage, is subject-verb (SV), verb-object (VO) or, still, in a subsequent period, subject-object-verb (SOV) (Hoffmeister, 1978Hoffmeister, R. (1978). Word order in the acquisition of ASL. Ms. Boston University.; Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.). This lack of the subject/object in productions may indicate the marking of the pro-drop parameter, i.e.: reduction/omission of the pronouns, when they can be pragmatically inferred (Lillo-Martin, 1986Lillo-Martin, D. (1986). Parameter setting: evidence from use, acquisition, and breakdown in American Sign Language. [Tese de Doutorado]. University of California, San Diego. University Microfilms International. Ann Arbor. Michigan.; Quadros, 1997Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem. Artmed.).

There are two verb classes in sign languages (Meier, 1987Meier, R. (1987). Elicited imitation of verb agreement in american sign language: iconically or morphologically determined? Journal of Memory and Language, 6(3), 362-376. Doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90119-7.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)901...
; Quadros, 1997Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem. Artmed.; Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.; Silva, 2010Silva, R. (2010). Língua gestual e bilinguismo na educação da criança surda. In Coelho, O. Um copo vazio está cheio de ar: assim é a surdez, (pp.101-145). Livpsic.): those that present agreement and, therefore, can be flexed (e.g.: GIVE, TELL, ASK, HELP); and those that present lexical and phonological limitations and, therefore, do not incorporate spatial points, also known as simple verbs, i.e., those that do not present any agreement, such as verbs anchored to the human body (e.g..: THINK, KNOW, LIKE). This suggests that deaf children, during sign language acquisition, should also adopt two different strategies in marking grammatical relations: word order and the incorporation of spatial points or pronouns, the second of which involves the process of verbal agreement which directly depends on the acquisition of the pronominal system, that is, the establishment of points in space (Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.).

Despite the fact that, at this stage, children use “pointing” to refer or reach someone/something, and the apparent relationship between the form and meaning of this action (the act of pointing represents the LGP pronouns), this pronominal system is used unconsciously, without understanding the pronouns referring to the linguistic system (Lillo-Martin et al., 1998Lillo-Martin, D., Mathur, G., & Quadros, R. (1998). Acquisition of verb agreement in asl and libras: a cross-linguistic study. Abstracts of the sixth international conference on theoretical issues in sign language research (pp. 12-15). Gallaudet University.; Quadros et al. 2001Quadros, R., Lillo-Martin, D., & Mathur, G. (2001). O que a aquisição da linguagem em crianças surdas tem a dizer sobre o estágio de infinitivos opcionais? Letras de Hoje, 36(3), 391-398.). At this stage, children, when referring to themselves, resort to pointing to a receiver, and there is a direct correlation between this frequent error, observable in all children (hearing or deaf), and the process of language acquisition (Petitto, 1987Petitto, L. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27(1), 1-52. Doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90034-5;
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)900...
). The naming of objects is only done in situations of immediate context, not yet making the syntactic use of space.

Between 2 and 3 years of age - stage of multiple combinations - the vocabulary explosion takes place, both in terms of gesture production and comprehension: the child talks about what it is doing, identifies things, describes people and objects, highlighting their characteristics, and starts using short sentences (Quadros, 1997Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem. Artmed.). The child begins to make some derivational distinctions (e.g.: the distinction between the gesture SITING and CHAIR (Lillo-Martin, 1986Lillo-Martin, D. (1986). Parameter setting: evidence from use, acquisition, and breakdown in American Sign Language. [Tese de Doutorado]. University of California, San Diego. University Microfilms International. Ann Arbor. Michigan.), but does not yet use pronominal identification to refer to people/things that are not present (Quadros, 1997Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem. Artmed.), using only nouns that are not associated with points in space (Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.).

If the referent is present in the discourse location, the child makes consistent use of the pronominal system and spatial cues, resorting to pointing that also relates to the systems of determiners and modifiers, pluralization, and verbal modulation (Hoffmeister, 1978Hoffmeister, R. (1978). Word order in the acquisition of ASL. Ms. Boston University.). Although with some errors (absence of referents), the pronominal system and verbal agreement with referents present in the context develop around the age of 3 (Petitto, 1987Petitto, L. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27(1), 1-52. Doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90034-5;
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)900...
; Quadros, 1997Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem. Artmed.; Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.), while difficulties are observed in verbal inflection (Klima & Bellugi, 1979Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Harvard University Press. ISBN 9780674807969.) and the use of verbs as having a single verbal class (Meier, 1987Meier, R. (1987). Elicited imitation of verb agreement in american sign language: iconically or morphologically determined? Journal of Memory and Language, 6(3), 362-376. Doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90119-7.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)901...
). Initially, the placement of points in space is performed inconsistently, with no association between location and reference, also complexifying verbal agreement (Meier, 1987Meier, R. (1987). Elicited imitation of verb agreement in american sign language: iconically or morphologically determined? Journal of Memory and Language, 6(3), 362-376. Doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90119-7.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)901...
). Lillo-Martin (1986)Lillo-Martin, D. (1986). Parameter setting: evidence from use, acquisition, and breakdown in American Sign Language. [Tese de Doutorado]. University of California, San Diego. University Microfilms International. Ann Arbor. Michigan. questions the iconicity (relation between form-meaning, referent-referent) of sign languages - visual-spatial and not auditory-oral - as well as its relation in the process of language acquisition, despite pointing out that the acquisition of the pronominal system and verbal agreement are carried out in the same way as in oral languages (Meier, 1986Meier, R. (1987). Elicited imitation of verb agreement in american sign language: iconically or morphologically determined? Journal of Memory and Language, 6(3), 362-376. Doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90119-7.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)901...
).

Finally, in the stage of the use of referents, from 5, 6-6, 6 years, the child reports simple and complex facts, past or present (Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.), in a long conversation, and with appropriate spatial representation regarding pronouns, correct use of syntactic function and consistent use of global morphological agreement (Amaral, 2006Amaral, A. (2006). Perspetivas teóricas na aquisição da linguagem em crianças surdas. In M. Bispo, A. Couto, M. Clara, & L. Clara (org.), O Gesto e a Palavra I: Antologia de textos sobre a surdez (pp. 109-150). Lisboa: Caminho. ISBN: 9789722117913.; Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.). The competence to communicate is matured around the age of 7, and the linguistic use and acquisition of space in sign language (Brazilian sign language) implies (Klima & Bellugi, 1979Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Harvard University Press. ISBN 9780674807969.; Silva, 1999Silva, M. (1999). A construção do sentido da escrita do sujeito surdo. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, São Paulo. ): information regarding the generalized differences of the place where the gesture is performed; the establishment of different spatial points; the identification of spatial location in a conscious way, and the use of it in the speech in a contrasting way. The speech is increasingly coherent and clear, recognizing the complexity of grammatical structures (Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.).

Sign languages have linguistic aspects, in the visual-spatial scope, equivalent to oral languages (Bellugi et al., 1989Bellugi, U., Poizner, H., & Klima, E. (1989). Language modality and the brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 12(10), 380-388. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90076-3.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(89)900...
; Stokoe et al. 1976Stokoe, W., Casterline, D., & Carl, A. (1976). A Dictionary of American Sign language on linguistic principles. New Edition, Listok Press.), denoting an analysis at all levels of linguistics, including cherology awareness, which, as the central theme of this article, will be described next.

Cherology awareness

Studies on oral language phonological awareness are extensive, in that the relationship between this ability and success in learning to read an alphabetic code is evident (Freitas et al. 2007Freitas, M., Alves, D., & Costa, T. (2007). PNEP – O conhecimento da língua: desenvolver a consciência fonológica. Ministério da Educação. ISBN: 978-972-742-269-2..; Santos et al., 2016Santos, I., Melo, M., & Roazzi, A. (2016). Consciência fonológica e alfabetização em crianças brasileiras: como esta relação tem evoluído? Iniciação Científica Cesumar, 18(2), 211-221. Doi: 10.17765/1518-1243.2016v18n2p211-221" 10.17765/1518-1243.2016v18n2p211-221.
https://doi.org/10.17765/1518-1243.2016v...
). This awareness is also a valuable tool in the intervention to promote the acquisition of writing (Freitas, 2004Freitas, C. (2004). Sobre a consciência fonológica. In R. Lamprecht, Aquisição fonológica do português: perfil de desenvolvimento e subsídios para a terapia (pp. 177-192). Artmed.; Freitas et al. 2007Freitas, M., Alves, D., & Costa, T. (2007). PNEP – O conhecimento da língua: desenvolver a consciência fonológica. Ministério da Educação. ISBN: 978-972-742-269-2..).

In LGP, cherology awareness, equated to phonological awareness in the Portuguese language, is a skill or metalinguistic awareness that involves knowledge of the formal features of language (Nóbrega 2019Nóbrega, V. (2019). Línguas de sinais: abordagem teóricas e aplicadas. Revista Leitura., 1(57). http://www.seer.ufal.br/index.php/revistaleitura/article/download/2657/2874.
http://www.seer.ufal.br/index.php/revist...
; Oliveira, 2015Oliveira, J. (2015). Análise descritiva da estrutura querológica de unidades terminológicas do glossário Letras-Libras. [Tese de Doutorado,. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Estudos de Tradução]. Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Santa Catarina.; Stokoe, 1960Stokoe, W. (1960). Sign and Culture: A reader for students of American Sign Language. Listok Press, Silver Spring, MD.). However, addressing the structure of this strand in LGP is still a major challenge due to the lack of studies in the area (Correia, 2009Correia, I. (2009). O parâmetro expressão na Língua Gestual Portuguesa: unidade suprassegmental. Exedra, Revista Científica da Escola Superior de Educação de Coimbra, 1, 57-68.), with terminological issues being one of the problems. Although both types of awareness - phonological and cherology - are equated, it should be understood that the former uses sound as expression (fonos from the Greek means sound), and the latter uses the hand to express itself (kiros from the Greek means hand). Therefore, it is preferable to adopt the second terminology when referring to sign language (Correia, 2009Correia, I. (2009). O parâmetro expressão na Língua Gestual Portuguesa: unidade suprassegmental. Exedra, Revista Científica da Escola Superior de Educação de Coimbra, 1, 57-68.; Stokoe, 1960Stokoe, W. (1960). Sign and Culture: A reader for students of American Sign Language. Listok Press, Silver Spring, MD.). Thus, in this study, in order to meet the visual-hand modality of language, we follow Stokoe’s (1960)Stokoe, W. (1960). Sign and Culture: A reader for students of American Sign Language. Listok Press, Silver Spring, MD. proposal, using cherology awareness that derives from chereme, the minimal unit of visual languages, rather than phonological awareness, which refers to phonemes, the minimal units of oral languages. This option only intends to adjust to the aforementioned etymological root of both terms and their adequacy to a language that has only a visual-motor modality and not oral-auditory.

The development of phonological/cherology ability begins early on, showing manifestations throughout language development, and progresses throughout childhood (Sim-Sim, 1998Sim-Sim, I. (1998). Desenvolvimento da linguagem. Universidade Aberta.). Its stages depend on the child’s cognitive and intellectual development, on the linguistic experiences provided, on formal exposure to the alphabetic system of their language (Freitas, 2004Freitas, C. (2004). Sobre a consciência fonológica. In R. Lamprecht, Aquisição fonológica do português: perfil de desenvolvimento e subsídios para a terapia (pp. 177-192). Artmed.), and also, in the case of LGP, on the articulatory system, i.e., on fine motor skills (Karnopp, 1999Karnopp, L. (1999). Aquisição fonológica da língua brasileira de Sinais: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda. [Dissertação de Doutoramento]. Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre.).

Speech acquisition, under normal circumstances, happens naturally and spontaneously (Freitas et al., 2007Freitas, M., Alves, D., & Costa, T. (2007). PNEP – O conhecimento da língua: desenvolver a consciência fonológica. Ministério da Educação. ISBN: 978-972-742-269-2..; Sim-Sim, 1998Sim-Sim, I. (1998). Desenvolvimento da linguagem. Universidade Aberta.); however, faculties such as reading and writing require “training the speech chain segmentation ability” (Freitas et al., 2007Freitas, M., Alves, D., & Costa, T. (2007). PNEP – O conhecimento da língua: desenvolver a consciência fonológica. Ministério da Educação. ISBN: 978-972-742-269-2.., p.7, free translation), such as segmenting sentences into words, words into syllables, and syllables into the several sounds that compose them. It is also important, in the view of the same authors, to realize that a language is made up of minimal linguistic units (speech sounds or segments), and the characters of the alphabet, in writing, represent these minimal units.

According to the Natural Phonology Theory, phonological processes are innate, natural and universal (Lima & Queiroga, 2007Lima, L., & Queiroga, B. (2007). Aquisição fonológica em crianças com antecedentes de desnutrição. Revista CEFAC, 9(1), 13-20. Doi: 10.1590/S1516-18462007000100003.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-1846200700...
), and can also be identified in visual-spatial modality languages, manifesting themselves by the alteration of one or more constituents of the gesture, so it can be said that a gesture comprises cherology processes (Guimarães & Campello, 2018Guimarães, C., & Campello, A. (2018). “Trocas nos sinais”: caracterização de processos fonológicos ocorridos durante a aquisição de Libras por pré-escolares surdos. Audiology Communication Research, 23, 1-6. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1922.
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1...
).

For Carroll et al. (2003)Carroll, J., Snowling, M., Hulme, C., & Stevenson, J. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in pre-school children. Developmental Psychology, 5(39) 913–923., the development of phonological awareness consists of a progression from large units (words) to the awareness of small units (phonemes). Similarly, in LGP, through the contribution of Stokoe (1965)Stokoe, W. (1960). Sign and Culture: A reader for students of American Sign Language. Listok Press, Silver Spring, MD., it was found that the sign should not be seen as a whole, but rather, made up of discrete and arbitrary parts. Thus, when LGP is the mother tongue of the deaf child, initially the awareness happens with the use of large units (gestures) and only at a later stage the child begins to be aware of the use and existence of minimal units, i.e., the parameters of LGP (configuration, place of articulation, movement, palm orientation and facial expression). Correia (2009)Correia, I. (2009). O parâmetro expressão na Língua Gestual Portuguesa: unidade suprassegmental. Exedra, Revista Científica da Escola Superior de Educação de Coimbra, 1, 57-68., comparing LP to LGP, illustrated the idea through the following example: considering the word “pato”, composed of four phonemes /p/a/t/u/, one sees that by combining it with other minimal units, a new word emerges (e.g: /p/a/ta/). The same happens with the alteration, absence of one of them or even the presence of a new unit. Also in LGP the gesture is composed of several discrete units - gesture parameters - that will define the overall meaning of the gesture (Correia, 2009Correia, I. (2009). O parâmetro expressão na Língua Gestual Portuguesa: unidade suprassegmental. Exedra, Revista Científica da Escola Superior de Educação de Coimbra, 1, 57-68.). As can be seen in the case of the sign words mulher (Figure 1) and difícil (Figure 2), which, although composed of the same hand configuration - “indicar” - have different meanings.

Figure 1
Gesture for MULHER
Figure 2
Gesture for DIFÍCIL

From birth, children are unconsciously sensitive to sounds or cheremes, in an auditory or visual way, respectively, revealing an epiphonological or epi-cherology behavior, which translates into the ability of early discrimination of sounds/cheremes (Basso, 2006Basso, F. (2006). A estimulação da consciência fonológica e a sua repercussão no processo de aprendizagem da lecto-escrita. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, Brasil..).

According to Freitas and collaborators (2007)Freitas, M., Alves, D., & Costa, T. (2007). PNEP – O conhecimento da língua: desenvolver a consciência fonológica. Ministério da Educação. ISBN: 978-972-742-269-2.., the process of analyzing words in their oral segments, or gestures in their minimal segments (cheremes), is a slow and difficult process, beginning at a pre-conscious level, i.e., with a phonological or cherology sensitivity, showing only functional knowledge (implicit awareness), progressing to its awareness, revealing meta-phonological or meta-cherology behaviors (explicit awareness).

The variability in exposure and experience with sign languages is quite considerable, so that most deaf children may show a process of delay in their acquisition, often with a first exposure limited to pre-school or primary school. Also, the number of deaf adults whose initial experience was based on oral education, and therefore learned/acquired their natural language as adolescents, is significant (Corina et al., 2014Corina, D., Hafer, S., & Keaarnean, W. (2014). Phonological awareness for american sign language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 19(4), 530-45. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enu023.
https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enu023...
). The authors examined the sensitivity to the structural parameters of gestures (minimal units) during sign language learning, showing that those who acquire sign languages late show lower efficiency, regarding the decoding of cherology forms, corroborating Guimarães and Campello (2018)Guimarães, C., & Campello, A. (2018). “Trocas nos sinais”: caracterização de processos fonológicos ocorridos durante a aquisição de Libras por pré-escolares surdos. Audiology Communication Research, 23, 1-6. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1922.
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1...
.

The relationship between learning to read in an alphabetic code has been studied by McQuarrie and Abbott (2013)McQuarrie, L., & Abbott, M. (2013). Bilingual deaf students’ phonological awareness in ASL and reading skills in english. Sign Language Studies, 14(1), 80-100. Doi: 10.1353/sls.2013.0028.
https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.2013.0028...
, and significant correlations have been demonstrated between ASL cherology awareness and reading performance in a second language, in this case English. These authors further argue that having a strong phonological (or cherology) base is more important than the modality it takes (oral or sign). Holmer et al. (2016)Holmer, E., Heimann, M., & Rudner, M. (2016). Evidence of an association between sign language phonological awareness and word reading in deaf and hard-ofhearing children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 48, 145-159. Doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.008.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.0...
assessed the Swedish Sign Language cherology awareness of 13 deaf children, and word reading in (oral) Swedish, and developed the Cross-modal Phonological Awareness Test, and a Swedish phonological awareness test. The results showed that deaf children who are more aware of the cherology of the sign language to which they are exposed have an easier time reading words in the oral language environment.

In the American Sign Language (ASL) setting, Corina et al. (2014)Corina, D., Hafer, S., & Keaarnean, W. (2014). Phonological awareness for american sign language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 19(4), 530-45. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enu023.
https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enu023...
investigated the relationship between ASL cherology awareness and English phonological awareness. A total of 87 deaf people participated in the study, divided according to their exposure to ASL into three groups: ASL as a mother tongue; exposed before the age of 8; and exposed after the age of 8. To carry out the study, the authors assessed ASL cherology awareness and compared it with English phonological awareness. The results showed that deaf people, exposed early to ASL, performed better on the first test. They also found a positive correlation between ASL cherology awareness and performance on the English phonological awareness test. Thus, one may wonder about the possible correlation between the cherology awareness of a sign language and the reading ability of a second language, such as written Portuguese.

In this follow-up, Cruz (2018)Cruz, C. (2018). Consciência fonológica da língua de sinais: implicações na linguagem e na leitura. Revel, edição especial, 15, 63-82. ISSN 1678-8931. clarifies that late exposure to the natural language of deaf children may prove to be a detriment in the language acquisition process. In addition, it is also pointed out that the learning of writing before an alphabetic code is influenced by this exposure, since deaf children with a late exposure learn a second language (Portuguese, in this case), through an alphabetic code (direct relationship with the sounds), supported by the low knowledge of their first language (sign language). Cruz (2018)Cruz, C. (2018). Consciência fonológica da língua de sinais: implicações na linguagem e na leitura. Revel, edição especial, 15, 63-82. ISSN 1678-8931. highlights the importance and need for these children to be monitored by specialists and proficient in their sign language, to prevent and identify deviations in the language acquisition process at different language levels.

In this regard, the school role in promoting phonological awareness and, in the case of LGP, cherology awareness, involves promoting the development of sensitivity to the phonic (or cherology) aspects of the language “with the aim of promoting phonological awareness, understood as the ability to identify and manipulate oral units” (Freitas et al., 2007Freitas, M., Alves, D., & Costa, T. (2007). PNEP – O conhecimento da língua: desenvolver a consciência fonológica. Ministério da Educação. ISBN: 978-972-742-269-2.., p.8, free translation). The authors also mentioned that, through systematicity and consistency, this ability should be stimulated from an early age in all children, thus promoting academic success.

Crume (2013)Crume, K. (2013). Teachers perceptions of promoting sign language phonological awareness in na ASL/English bilingual program. Journal of deaf studies and deaf education, 18, 464-488. doi: 10.1093/deafed/ent023;
https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ent023...
verified, through a questionnaire, how ASI teachers drew on their practices and beliefs to promote the learning of cherology awareness of this language in ASL/English bilingual education. Results showed that these teachers exploit ASL cherology awareness as a strategy in promoting their students’ literacy. The respondents pointed out that, in addition to the need for the use of varied strategies, a good knowledge of the structure of ASL provides a solid ground for its development, helping deaf students to make more connections between words and language.

Thus, and although the relationship between reading ability and cherology awareness is not yet proven, Cruz (2018)Cruz, C. (2018). Consciência fonológica da língua de sinais: implicações na linguagem e na leitura. Revel, edição especial, 15, 63-82. ISSN 1678-8931. states that the construction of cherology knowledge of a sign language largely depends on the acquisition and solid knowledge of the language, making it an object of thought and reflection - metalinguistic ability. Thus, Cruz (2018)Cruz, C. (2018). Consciência fonológica da língua de sinais: implicações na linguagem e na leitura. Revel, edição especial, 15, 63-82. ISSN 1678-8931. concludes that early access to the sign language of their country, as well as strategies in promoting the teaching of cherology, may significantly contribute to success in reading a second language.

Acquisition of cherology awareness

Studies on cherology acquisition are still scarce, with those conducted in ASL and, occasionally, in sign language standing out. Siedlecki and Bonvillian (1993)Siedlecki, T., & Bonvillian, J. (1993). Location, handshape and movement: young children’s acquisition of the formational aspects of american sign language. Sign language studies, 78, 31-52. Doi: 10.1353/sls.1993.0016.
https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.1993.0016...
establish three measures of cherology awareness: the order of acquisition, with place of articulation being the first to be acquired, followed by movement and hand configuration; production accuracy, with average values of 83% for place of articulation, 61% for movement and 49% for hand configuration; and frequency of production. Similarly, Marentette (1995)Marentette, P. (1995). It’s in her hands: A case study of the emergence of phonology in American Sign Language. ([Dissertação de Mestrado, Department of Psychology]. McGill University, Montreal. found that the acquisition of the articulation place parameter was the most correctly produced, and only 16 of 182 substitutions were not articulation sites close to the correct target. According to the author, all this evidence reflected in an early age is explained by the language modality, which is visually produced, and to the body scheme.

Karnopp (1997)Karnopp, L. (1997). Aquisição fonológica da língua brasileira de Sinais. Letras de Hoje, 32(4), 147-162. presented the linguistic description of the process of acquiring the place of articulation, through the following leveled order regarding the acquired points: 1st) neutral space, trunk, chin and forehead; 2nd) hand and cheek; 3rd) mid-face, wrist, neck, head; 4th) forearm; 5th) upper arm. On the other hand, and taking into account movement acquisition, Siedlecki and Bonvillian (1993)Siedlecki, T., & Bonvillian, J. (1993). Location, handshape and movement: young children’s acquisition of the formational aspects of american sign language. Sign language studies, 78, 31-52. Doi: 10.1353/sls.1993.0016.
https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.1993.0016...
found correct production in 58% (under 14 months of age), 63% (between 14 and 15 months), and 62% (over 16 months) for this parameter.

Siedlecki and Bonvillian (1993)Siedlecki, T., & Bonvillian, J. (1993). Location, handshape and movement: young children’s acquisition of the formational aspects of american sign language. Sign language studies, 78, 31-52. Doi: 10.1353/sls.1993.0016.
https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.1993.0016...
highlight an increasing development of acquisition of the hand configuration parameter, relating it to the correct production and the number of different productions according to age. Marentette (1995)Marentette, P. (1995). It’s in her hands: A case study of the emergence of phonology in American Sign Language. ([Dissertação de Mestrado, Department of Psychology]. McGill University, Montreal. found a large production of different hand configurations, although most consisted of only three configurations present in ASL: number 5, number 1 and letter A, given their simpler linguistic representation (Karnopp, 1999Karnopp, L. (1999). Aquisição fonológica da língua brasileira de Sinais: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda. [Dissertação de Doutoramento]. Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre.; Siedlecki & Bonvillian, 1993Siedlecki, T., & Bonvillian, J. (1993). Location, handshape and movement: young children’s acquisition of the formational aspects of american sign language. Sign language studies, 78, 31-52. Doi: 10.1353/sls.1993.0016.
https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.1993.0016...
). Since configurations vary between sign languages, in Table 1 we tried to relate the configurations found in previous studies approached using the LGP configurations.

In a pioneering study on the acquisition of LGP, between 10 months and 2 years, with a profoundly deaf child, daughter of deaf parents, we found certain errors in the appropriation of cherology category (Carmo, 2010Carmo, P. (2010). Aquisição da língua gestual portuguesa: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda dos 10 aos 24 meses. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisboa.): awareness of the minimum unit of the hand configuration with a percentage of 43.02%; location (9.88%), movement (6.40%), direction (2.91%) and place of articulation of the hand (1.74%). These results are in line with other studies, with deaf children between 2 and 12 years old, daughters of deaf parents, in which the hand configuration parameter shows the highest rate of alterations (Guimarães & Campello, 2018Guimarães, C., & Campello, A. (2018). “Trocas nos sinais”: caracterização de processos fonológicos ocorridos durante a aquisição de Libras por pré-escolares surdos. Audiology Communication Research, 23, 1-6. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1922.
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1...
; Karnopp, 1997Karnopp, L. (1997). Aquisição fonológica da língua brasileira de Sinais. Letras de Hoje, 32(4), 147-162., 1999), and with deaf children between 3 and 7 years old, in which the hand configuration parameter was the most affected, followed by location, movement, and direction, with some gestures revealing more than one alteration in the phonological process (Guimarães & Campello, 2018Guimarães, C., & Campello, A. (2018). “Trocas nos sinais”: caracterização de processos fonológicos ocorridos durante a aquisição de Libras por pré-escolares surdos. Audiology Communication Research, 23, 1-6. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1922.
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1...
).

Regarding semantic categories, Carmo (2010)Carmo, P. (2010). Aquisição da língua gestual portuguesa: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda dos 10 aos 24 meses. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisboa. found in a case study that the child performed 145 different gestures, and the first and most frequent configuration to be evoked was the “Open Hand”, since it tends to be easier to perform. After the third month of study, a new configuration appeared in the child’s cherology field (Letter “S”), with total manipulation of the hand, even totally closed. Only after the fourth month of observation, it was possible to verify the non-use of the hand in its entirety and, over the following months, totaling 32 configurations, Carmo (2010)Carmo, P. (2010). Aquisição da língua gestual portuguesa: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda dos 10 aos 24 meses. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisboa. verified a greater diversification and use of configurations.

Table 1
Relation between ASL and LGP configurations. (Adapted from Karnopp, 1999Karnopp, L. (1999). Aquisição fonológica da língua brasileira de Sinais: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda. [Dissertação de Doutoramento]. Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre., p.182; Carmo, 2010Carmo, P. (2010). Aquisição da língua gestual portuguesa: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda dos 10 aos 24 meses. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisboa.)

The author concluded that the cherology components of the movement were mostly performed correctly and efficiently: 100% correct in the gestures that had as a minimum unit the movements of circling, fingering, twisting and rubbing (Carmo, 2010Carmo, P. (2010). Aquisição da língua gestual portuguesa: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda dos 10 aos 24 meses. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisboa.). Similarly, all showed a high percentage of efficiency in the execution of directions, with the exception of the upward movement direction (66.67%). In the location parameter there were no major changes (Carmo, 2010Carmo, P. (2010). Aquisição da língua gestual portuguesa: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda dos 10 aos 24 meses. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisboa.).

The cherology assessment of Cruz (2008)Cruz, C. (2008). Proposta de instrumento de avaliação da consciência fonológica, parâmetro configuração de mão, para crianças surdas utentes da língua de sinais brasileira. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul.. argues that it is necessary to promote good gesture formation in the execution of gestures, so its items are pooled taking into account the evolution of acquisition: (a) gestures produced with one hand, with one or two hand configurations; (b) gestures produced with both hands, with the same configuration, occurring, mostly, the condition of symmetry; and (c) gestures produced with both hands, being the configurations different, that is, one hand is dominant, and the other serves only as a point of articulation (non-dominant). In oral language, errors in phonological processes may be (Guimarães & Campello, 2018Guimarães, C., & Campello, A. (2018). “Trocas nos sinais”: caracterização de processos fonológicos ocorridos durante a aquisição de Libras por pré-escolares surdos. Audiology Communication Research, 23, 1-6. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1922.
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1...
): substitution processes - in which there is a change/contrast in sublexical units, such as “sapo” for “tapo”; structural modifier processes - referring to the combination of phonemes in the formation of morphological and lexical units, such as “árvore” for “rávore”; and context-sensitive processes - in which there is reference to the substitutions of features or segments by others from the closest phonological context, such as “chupeta” for “chucha”. In the cherology component, Liddell and Johnson (1989)Liddell, S., & Johnson, R. (1989). American sign language: the phonological base. Sign Lang Stud. 64(1), 195-277. Doi: 10.1353/sls.1989.0027.
https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.1989.0027...
, pioneers in this study, classified the most frequent errors (Table 2) into: epenthesis, omission, metathesis, gemination, assimilation, reduction and perseveration, and anticipation (Guimarães & Campello, 2018Guimarães, C., & Campello, A. (2018). “Trocas nos sinais”: caracterização de processos fonológicos ocorridos durante a aquisição de Libras por pré-escolares surdos. Audiology Communication Research, 23, 1-6. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1922.
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1...
).

Table 2
Errors in cherology processes. (Adapted from Guimarães and Campello, 2018Guimarães, C., & Campello, A. (2018). “Trocas nos sinais”: caracterização de processos fonológicos ocorridos durante a aquisição de Libras por pré-escolares surdos. Audiology Communication Research, 23, 1-6. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1922.
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1...
)

Final remarks

Sign languages have linguistic aspects of visual-spatial nature, equivalent to oral languages (Bellugi et al., 1989Bellugi, U., Poizner, H., & Klima, E. (1989). Language modality and the brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 12(10), 380-388. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90076-3.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(89)900...
; Stokoe et al., 1976Stokoe, W., Casterline, D., & Carl, A. (1976). A Dictionary of American Sign language on linguistic principles. New Edition, Listok Press.), and as a natural language of deaf children should be taught early, as a natural process (Quadros & Cruz, 2011Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação. Artmed.) in all school activities (Carmo et al., 2007Carmo, H., Martins, M., Morgado, M., & Estanqueiro, P. (2007). Programa curricular de língua gestual portuguesa educação pré-escolar e ensino básico. Ministério da Educação, Direcção-Geral de Inovação e de Desenvolvimento Curricular.). Despite the tendency for exposure to oral linguistic input (Batista, 2010Batista, M. (2010). Alunos surdos: aquisição da língua gestual e ensino da Língua Portuguesa. Exedra, 9, 1-12.) deaf children, when fully exposed to their natural and mother tongue (Lillo-Martin, 1986Lillo-Martin, D. (1986). Parameter setting: evidence from use, acquisition, and breakdown in American Sign Language. [Tese de Doutorado]. University of California, San Diego. University Microfilms International. Ann Arbor. Michigan.; Meier, 1987Meier, R. (1987). Elicited imitation of verb agreement in american sign language: iconically or morphologically determined? Journal of Memory and Language, 6(3), 362-376. Doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90119-7.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)901...
; Petitto, 1987Petitto, L. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27(1), 1-52. Doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90034-5;
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)900...
), develop grammatical rules in a similar way to hearing children in relation to oral language (Quadros et al., 2001Quadros, R., Lillo-Martin, D., & Mathur, G. (2001). O que a aquisição da linguagem em crianças surdas tem a dizer sobre o estágio de infinitivos opcionais? Letras de Hoje, 36(3), 391-398.).

Despite the differences at the cherology structural level of a visual language and phonological level of an oral language, it is found that the processing of this knowledge is similar (Cruz, 2018Cruz, C. (2018). Consciência fonológica da língua de sinais: implicações na linguagem e na leitura. Revel, edição especial, 15, 63-82. ISSN 1678-8931.). The child is sensitive to sounds or cheremes (in the case of Sign Languages) from birth, which translates into the ability of early discrimination of sounds/cheremes (Basso, 2006Basso, F. (2006). A estimulação da consciência fonológica e a sua repercussão no processo de aprendizagem da lecto-escrita. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, Brasil..). According to Freitas and collaborators (2007)Freitas, M., Alves, D., & Costa, T. (2007). PNEP – O conhecimento da língua: desenvolver a consciência fonológica. Ministério da Educação. ISBN: 978-972-742-269-2.., the process of gesture analysis in its minimal segments (cheremes) is a slow and difficult-to-access process, starting in a pre-conscious level, i.e., with the cherology sensitivity, showing only functional knowledge (implicit awareness), progressing to cherology awareness (explicit awareness). Thus, it can be concluded that this ability represents a very relevant milestone in the acquisition and development of sign language, since it is used from the beginning until the language matures. In addition, one should also take into account that there are other factors that influence language: culture, ethnicity, country, television, conversational peers, experiences, age of caregivers, multilingualism, school, socioeconomic status, among others (Hoff, 2006Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 55-88. doi :10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002...
).

Investigations on the implication of age of language acquisition on cherology awareness and sign language comprehension and production (Corina et al., 2014Corina, D., Hafer, S., & Keaarnean, W. (2014). Phonological awareness for american sign language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 19(4), 530-45. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enu023.
https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enu023...
; Crume, 2013Crume, K. (2013). Teachers perceptions of promoting sign language phonological awareness in na ASL/English bilingual program. Journal of deaf studies and deaf education, 18, 464-488. doi: 10.1093/deafed/ent023;
https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ent023...
; McQuarrie & Abott, 2013McQuarrie, L., & Abbott, M. (2013). Bilingual deaf students’ phonological awareness in ASL and reading skills in english. Sign Language Studies, 14(1), 80-100. Doi: 10.1353/sls.2013.0028.
https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.2013.0028...
) reinforce the importance of early acquisition of natural language by deaf children. Although studies are still pioneering, there seems to be a positive correlation between cherology awareness in reading an alphabetic code and the reading proficiency of a second language - written Portuguese (Cruz, 2018Cruz, C. (2018). Consciência fonológica da língua de sinais: implicações na linguagem e na leitura. Revel, edição especial, 15, 63-82. ISSN 1678-8931.). Then, it is fundamental to consider the assessment of language acquisition a necessary step in the teaching-learning of it (Cruz, 2018Cruz, C. (2018). Consciência fonológica da língua de sinais: implicações na linguagem e na leitura. Revel, edição especial, 15, 63-82. ISSN 1678-8931.). This idea is defended by Lima (2011)Lima, R. (2011). Fonologia infantil: aquisição, avaliação e intervenção. Almedina. ISBN: 9789724037837., who states that the evaluation of language development serves to perceive the level of the assessed individual, as well as the need for some kind of intervention.

The assessment, monitoring of the learning process and planning of interventions in the Portuguese language, more precisely in terms of cherology awareness, still has some gaps at the national level, namely in terms of: conceptual scope, with limited evidence of the developmental profile of sign language acquisition and reflection on the representativeness and significance of the selected items; and methodological scope, with a tendency for studies with small samples and without an examination of the main psychometric properties (content and construct validity, and reliability), with limited publication, which limits its usefulness. Moreover, most of the available instruments only assess partial aspects of cherology awareness: some use the discrimination of minimal pairs, others ask the respondent to perform the gesture, assessing their cherology proficiency, and still others assess only the parameters with greater focus on the hand configuration, movement and location parameter.

Thus, it could be stated that there is no instrument in the field of LGP to assess emerging language, which affects the transition between pre-linguistic and linguistic stages (Afonso 2011Afonso, M. (2011). Elementos para a construção de um teste de rastreio de perturbações do desenvolvimento sintático. [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Nova de Lisboa.). In this field, little attention is given to cherology awareness and its assessment, justifying further research on the topic.

Referências

  • Afonso, M. (2011). Elementos para a construção de um teste de rastreio de perturbações do desenvolvimento sintático [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Nova de Lisboa.
  • Amaral, A. (2006). Perspetivas teóricas na aquisição da linguagem em crianças surdas. In M. Bispo, A. Couto, M. Clara, & L. Clara (org.), O Gesto e a Palavra I: Antologia de textos sobre a surdez (pp. 109-150). Lisboa: Caminho. ISBN: 9789722117913.
  • Amaral, A., & Coutinho, A. (2002). A criança surda: educação e inserção social. Análise Psicológica, 3, 373-378. doi: 10.14417/ap.324.
    » https://doi.org/10.14417/ap.324
  • Amaral, I. (1999). Comunicação e linguagem. In F. Pereira (Cord.), O aluno surdo em contexto escolar. Apoios educativos (pp. 37-48). Ministério da Educação.
  • Augusto, M. (1995). Teoria gerativa e aquisição da linguagem. Sitientibus, 13, 115-120. http://www2.uefs.br/sitientibus/pdf/13/teoria_gerativa_e_aquisicao_da_linguagem.pdf
    » http://www2.uefs.br/sitientibus/pdf/13/teoria_gerativa_e_aquisicao_da_linguagem.pdf
  • Basso, F. (2006). A estimulação da consciência fonológica e a sua repercussão no processo de aprendizagem da lecto-escrita [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, Brasil..
  • Batista, J., Santiago, A., Almeida, D., Antunes, P., & Gaspar, R. (2011). Programa de Português L2 para alunos surdos Ministério da Educação, Direção-Geral de Inovação e Desenvolvimento Curricular.
  • Batista, M. (2010). Alunos surdos: aquisição da língua gestual e ensino da Língua Portuguesa. Exedra, 9, 1-12.
  • Bellugi, U., Poizner, H., & Klima, E. (1989). Language modality and the brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 12(10), 380-388. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90076-3.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(89)90076-3
  • Carmo, H., Martins, M., Morgado, M., & Estanqueiro, P. (2007). Programa curricular de língua gestual portuguesa educação pré-escolar e ensino básico Ministério da Educação, Direcção-Geral de Inovação e de Desenvolvimento Curricular.
  • Carmo, P. (2010). Aquisição da língua gestual portuguesa: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda dos 10 aos 24 meses [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisboa.
  • Carroll, J., Snowling, M., Hulme, C., & Stevenson, J. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in pre-school children. Developmental Psychology, 5(39) 913–923.
  • Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B.F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Language, 35(I), 26-58.
  • Chomsky, N. (2006). Language and mind Cambridge: University Press. ISBN: 9780521674935.
  • Corina, D., Hafer, S., & Keaarnean, W. (2014). Phonological awareness for american sign language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 19(4), 530-45. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enu023.
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enu023
  • Correia, I. (2009). O parâmetro expressão na Língua Gestual Portuguesa: unidade suprassegmental. Exedra, Revista Científica da Escola Superior de Educação de Coimbra, 1, 57-68.
  • Costa, D., Azambuja, L., & Nunes, M. (2002). Avaliação do desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor. In M. Nunes, & A. Marrone (Eds), Semiologia Neurologica (pp. 351-360), Porto Alegre.
  • Crume, K. (2013). Teachers perceptions of promoting sign language phonological awareness in na ASL/English bilingual program. Journal of deaf studies and deaf education, 18, 464-488. doi: 10.1093/deafed/ent023;
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ent023
  • Cruz, C. (2008). Proposta de instrumento de avaliação da consciência fonológica, parâmetro configuração de mão, para crianças surdas utentes da língua de sinais brasileira [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul..
  • Cruz, C. (2018). Consciência fonológica da língua de sinais: implicações na linguagem e na leitura. Revel, edição especial, 15, 63-82. ISSN 1678-8931.
  • Diário da República. (2018, 6 jul.). Decreto-Lei n.º 54/2018 Regime Jurídico da Educação Inclusiva (1.ª série, n. 129, pp. 2918-2928).
  • Fernandes, E. (2003). linguagem e surdez Artmed Editora.
  • Franco, M., Reis, M., & Gil, S. (2003). Domínio da comunicação, linguagem e fala. Perturbações específicas de linguagem em contexto escolar: fundamentos Ministério da Educação.
  • Freitas, C. (2004). Sobre a consciência fonológica. In R. Lamprecht, Aquisição fonológica do português: perfil de desenvolvimento e subsídios para a terapia (pp. 177-192). Artmed.
  • Freitas, M., Alves, D., & Costa, T. (2007). PNEP – O conhecimento da língua: desenvolver a consciência fonológica Ministério da Educação. ISBN: 978-972-742-269-2..
  • Geffner, D., Harry L., Freeman L., & Gaffney R. (1978). Speech and language assessment scales of deaf children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 11(2-3), 215-226. Doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(78)90014-X.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(78)90014-X
  • Guimarães, C., & Campello, A. (2018). “Trocas nos sinais”: caracterização de processos fonológicos ocorridos durante a aquisição de Libras por pré-escolares surdos. Audiology Communication Research, 23, 1-6. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1922.
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2017-1922
  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 55-88. doi :10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002
  • Hoffmeister, R. (1978). Word order in the acquisition of ASL Ms. Boston University.
  • Holmer, E., Heimann, M., & Rudner, M. (2016). Evidence of an association between sign language phonological awareness and word reading in deaf and hard-ofhearing children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 48, 145-159. Doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.008.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.008
  • Johnson, S., & Newport, E. (1989). Critical periods effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21(1), 60-99. Doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0
  • Karnopp, L. (1997). Aquisição fonológica da língua brasileira de Sinais. Letras de Hoje, 32(4), 147-162.
  • Karnopp, L. (1999). Aquisição fonológica da língua brasileira de Sinais: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda [Dissertação de Doutoramento]. Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre.
  • Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language Harvard University Press. ISBN 9780674807969.
  • Knudsen, E. (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1412-1425. Doi: 10.1162/0898929042304796.
    » https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042304796
  • Lane, H., Hoffmeister, R., & Bahan, B. (1996). A journey into the deaf world Dawn Sign Publishers.
  • Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language With Appendices by Noam Chomksy and Otto Marx. Wiley.
  • Liddell, S., & Johnson, R. (1989). American sign language: the phonological base. Sign Lang Stud. 64(1), 195-277. Doi: 10.1353/sls.1989.0027.
    » https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.1989.0027
  • Lillo-Martin, D. (1986). Parameter setting: evidence from use, acquisition, and breakdown in American Sign Language. [Tese de Doutorado]. University of California, San Diego. University Microfilms International. Ann Arbor. Michigan.
  • Lillo-Martin, D., Mathur, G., & Quadros, R. (1998). Acquisition of verb agreement in asl and libras: a cross-linguistic study. Abstracts of the sixth international conference on theoretical issues in sign language research (pp. 12-15). Gallaudet University.
  • Lima, L., & Queiroga, B. (2007). Aquisição fonológica em crianças com antecedentes de desnutrição. Revista CEFAC, 9(1), 13-20. Doi: 10.1590/S1516-18462007000100003.
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462007000100003
  • Lima, R. (2000). Linguagem infantil Da normalidade à patologia APPCDM Distrital de Braga.
  • Lima, R. (2011). Fonologia infantil: aquisição, avaliação e intervenção Almedina. ISBN: 9789724037837.
  • Marentette, P. (1995). It’s in her hands: A case study of the emergence of phonology in American Sign Language ([Dissertação de Mestrado, Department of Psychology]. McGill University, Montreal.
  • McQuarrie, L., & Abbott, M. (2013). Bilingual deaf students’ phonological awareness in ASL and reading skills in english. Sign Language Studies, 14(1), 80-100. Doi: 10.1353/sls.2013.0028.
    » https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.2013.0028
  • Meier, P. (2002). The acquisition of verb agreement: pointing out arguments for the linguistic status of agreement in signed languages. Directions in sign language acquisition, 115-141. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.2.08mei.
    » https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.2.08mei
  • Meier, R. (1987). Elicited imitation of verb agreement in american sign language: iconically or morphologically determined? Journal of Memory and Language, 6(3), 362-376. Doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90119-7.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)90119-7
  • Mineiro, A., Duarte, L., Carvalho, P., Tebé, C., & Correia, M. (2008). Aspectos da polissemia nominal em língua gestual portuguesa. Revista Polissema, 8, 37-56.
  • Neves, D., & Miranda, M. (s.d.). A criança surda e o desenvolvimento da linguagem. Educere ISSN 2176-1396.
  • Neville, H., & Bavelier, D. (2002). Human brain plasticity: evidence from sensory deprivation and altered language experience. Progress in Brain Research, 138, 177-188. Doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(02)38078-6.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(02)38078-6
  • Newport, E. (1988). Constraints on learning and their role in language acquisition: studies on the acquisition of american sign language. Language Science, 10(1), 147-172. Doi: 10.1016/0388-0001(88)90010-1.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0388-0001(88)90010-1
  • Newport, E. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11-28. Doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2;
    » https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2
  • Nóbrega, V. (2019). Línguas de sinais: abordagem teóricas e aplicadas. Revista Leitura., 1(57). http://www.seer.ufal.br/index.php/revistaleitura/article/download/2657/2874
    » http://www.seer.ufal.br/index.php/revistaleitura/article/download/2657/2874
  • Oliveira, J. (2015). Análise descritiva da estrutura querológica de unidades terminológicas do glossário Letras-Libras [Tese de Doutorado,. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Estudos de Tradução]. Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Santa Catarina.
  • Petitto, A., & Marantette, P. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251, 1483 - 1496.
  • Petitto, L. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27(1), 1-52. Doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90034-5;
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)90034-5
  • Petitto, L., Zatorre, R., Gauna, K., Nikelski, E., Dostie, D., & Evns, A. (2000). Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: implications for the neural basis of human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of SSciences of the United States of America, 97(25), 13961-13966. Doi: 10.1073./pnas.97.25.13961.
    » https://doi.org/10.1073./pnas.97.25.13961
  • Quadros, R. (1997). Educação de Surdos: a aquisição da linguagem Artmed.
  • Quadros, R., & Cruz, C. (2011). Língua de sinais: instrumentos de avaliação Artmed.
  • Quadros, R., Lillo-Martin, D., & Mathur, G. (2001). O que a aquisição da linguagem em crianças surdas tem a dizer sobre o estágio de infinitivos opcionais? Letras de Hoje, 36(3), 391-398.
  • Quadros, R., Cruz, R., & Pizzio, A. (2010). Desenvolvimento da língua de sinais: a determinação do input Trabalho apresentado no 8º congresso internacional da ISAPL (Society of applied psycholinguistics). Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul.
  • Reis, V. (1997). A linguagem e seus efeitos no desenvolvimento cognitivo e emocional da criança surda. Espaço Informativo Técnico Científico do INES, 6, 23-38.
  • Rubio, S., & Queiroz, S. (2014). A aquisição da linguagem e integração social: a libras como formadora de identidade do surdo. Revista Eletrônica Saberes da Educação, 5(1), 1-15.
  • Santos, I., Melo, M., & Roazzi, A. (2016). Consciência fonológica e alfabetização em crianças brasileiras: como esta relação tem evoluído? Iniciação Científica Cesumar, 18(2), 211-221. Doi: 10.17765/1518-1243.2016v18n2p211-221" 10.17765/1518-1243.2016v18n2p211-221.
    » https://doi.org/10.17765/1518-1243.2016v18n2p211-221
  • Schirmer, C., Fontoura, D., & Nunes, M. (2004). Distúrbios da aquisição da linguagem e da aprendizagem. Jornal de Pediatria Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria 80(2), 95-103. Doi: 10.1590/S0021-75572004000300012.
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0021-75572004000300012
  • Siedlecki, T., & Bonvillian, J. (1993). Location, handshape and movement: young children’s acquisition of the formational aspects of american sign language. Sign language studies, 78, 31-52. Doi: 10.1353/sls.1993.0016.
    » https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.1993.0016
  • Silva, M. (1999). A construção do sentido da escrita do sujeito surdo [Dissertação de Mestrado]. Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, São Paulo.
  • Silva, R. (2010). Língua gestual e bilinguismo na educação da criança surda. In Coelho, O. Um copo vazio está cheio de ar: assim é a surdez, (pp.101-145). Livpsic.
  • Sim-Sim, I. (1998). Desenvolvimento da linguagem Universidade Aberta.
  • Sim-Sim, I., Silva, A., & Nunes, C. (2008). Linguagem e Comunicação no jardim de infância: textos de apoio para educadores de infância Ministério da Educação, Direção - Geral de Inovação e Desenvolvimento Curricular.
  • Singleton, J., & Newport, E. (1994). When learners surpass their models: the acquisition of american sign language from inconsistent input. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 370-407. Doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.05.001.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.05.001
  • Spinassé, P. (2006). Os conceitos língua materna, segunda língua e língua estrangeira e os falantes de línguas alóctones minoritárias no Sul do Brasil. Revista Contingentia, 1, 1-10.
  • Stokoe, W. (1960). Sign and Culture: A reader for students of American Sign Language Listok Press, Silver Spring, MD.
  • Stokoe, W., Casterline, D., & Carl, A. (1976). A Dictionary of American Sign language on linguistic principles New Edition, Listok Press.

Edited by

1
Responsible Editor: Alexandre Filordi de Carvalho. - https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4510-9440

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    11 Nov 2022
  • Date of issue
    2022

History

  • Received
    30 Mar 2021
  • Accepted
    10 June 2021
UNICAMP - Faculdade de Educação Av Bertrand Russel, 801, 13083-865 - Campinas SP/ Brasil, Tel.: (55 19) 3521-6707 - Campinas - SP - Brazil
E-mail: proposic@unicamp.br