Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

INFLUENCE OF ACTIVE GAMES AND PLAY ON CHILDREN’S COGNITION: A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW

INFLUÊNCIA DOS JOGOS E BRINCADEIRAS ATIVOS NA COGNIÇÃO DE CRIANÇAS: UMA REVISÃO SISTEMÁTICA

ABSTRACT

Games and play are strategies to assist in the cognitive development of children. However, there are still gaps about their effects and associations when it comes to children’s cognition. Thus, the objective is to synthesize the influence of active games and play on the cognition of children. The databases searched were: Lilacs, PsycINFO, Pubmed, Scielo and Scopus. Articles with observational or experimental design were included. Their quality was evaluated by means of RoB

2. Five studies were included, in accordance with the eligibility criteria. All studies had an experimental design, with two being conducted chronically, and three, acutely. Three studies showed positive effects of games and play on attention, reaction time, and executive functions: inhibitory control, logical reasoning, verbal factor, numerical factor, spatial factor, and general intelligence. Regarding the methodological quality of the studies, it is necessary to better describe and use the randomization allocation process, be more careful with data analysis, and avoid multiple assessments for a single variable. It is concluded that active games and play can generate a positive effect on the attention and executive functions of children. Further research is needed for an understanding of the relationship that parameters such as intensity, length and types of activities have with cognition.

Keywords:
Games and playthings; Cognition; Child

RESUMO

Jogos e brincadeiras são estratégias para auxiliar no desenvolvimento cognitivo do público infantil. Contudo ainda existem lacunas sobre os efeitos e associações dos jogos e brincadeiras ativos na cognição de crianças. Assim, o objetivo é sintetizar a influência dos jogos e brincadeiras ativos na cognição de crianças. As bases de dados foram: Lilacs, PsycINFO, Pubmed, Scielo e Scopus. Foram incluídos artigos com delineamento observacional ou experimental. A qualidade dos artigos foi avaliada com a RoB 2. Cinco estudos foram incluídos seguindo os critérios de elegibilidade. Todos os estudos apresentaram delineamento experimental, dois realizados de forma crônica e três de forma aguda. Três estudos apresentaram efeitos positivos dos jogos e brincadeiras na atenção, tempo de reação e funções executivas: controle inibitório, raciocínio lógico, fator verbal, fator numérico, fator espacial e inteligência geral. Quanto à qualidade metodológica dos estudos é necessário melhor descrição e utilização do processo de alocação da randomização, maior cuidado na análise de dados e evitar múltiplas avaliações para uma variável. Conclui-se que jogos e brincadeiras ativos podem gerar efeito positivo para a atenção e funções executivas de crianças. Mais estudos são necessários para compreensão da relação de parâmetros como intensidade, duração e tipos de atividades com a cognição.

Palavras-chave:
Jogos e brinquedos; Cognição; Criança

Introduction

Engaging in games and play is a very present experience in childhood and has a major share in children’s development1Nijhof SL, Vinkers CH, van Geelen SM, Duijff SN, Achterberg EJM, van der Net J, et al. Healthy play, better coping: the importance of play for the development of children in health and disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018; 95(95):421-9. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.024
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018...
. During these activities, they can be provided with various motor stimuli (e.g., walking, jumping, kicking, running, rolling), which contributes to children’s motor development2Lämmle C, Kobel S, Wartha O, Wirt T, Steinacker JM. Intervention effects of a school-based health promotion program on children’s motor skills. J. Public Health. 2016;24(3):185-92. DOI: 10.1007/s10389-016-0715-x
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10389-016-0715-...
. The interaction between motor skills and cognitive functions, such as tactile exploration or postural control challenges, also stimulates the Central Nervous System3Van der Fels IMJ, Te Wierike SCM, Hartman E, Elferink-Gemser MT, Smith J, Visscher C. The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4-16 year old typically developing children: a systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2015;18(6):697-703. DOI: 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.09.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2014.09....
. Furthermore, social stimulation is a constant presence in games and play, since children are always communicating and interacting, due to the goals and strategies of the activities4Cordazzo STD, Vieira ML. A brincadeira e suas implicações nos processos de aprendizagem e de desenvolvimento. Estud. Pesqui. Psicol. [Internet]. 2007[Acesso em 04 fev 2023];7(1). Disponível em: Disponível em: http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1808-42812007000100009.
http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?scr...
.

Thus, the stimuli arising from engagement in games and play can assist in the process of integral development in childhood, in terms of cognition, motricity and affection5Wallon H. Do ato ao pensamento: ensaio de psicologia comparada. 2nd ed. Vol. 2. 2008.. However, the habit of playing among children is nowadays less and less noticeable, due to factors such as lack of safety, scarcity of spaces for play, which can consequently affect their mental health6Gray PH. The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and adolescents. Am. J. Play [Internet]. 2011Jan 1 [Acesso em 04 fev 2023];3(4):443-63. Disponível em:Disponível em:https://wowgym.bg/wp- content/uploads/2014/03/3-4-article-gray-decline-of-play.pdf
https://wowgym.bg/wp- content/uploads/20...
.

On the other hand, research has used games and play in various ways, with differences in their approach and purposes, but with the aim of understanding their possible contributions to children’s lives. Thus, we can highlight their application to: Cognitive therapy; Teaching strategy for school content; Active electronic games; Behavioral assessment; Engagement in physical activity7Hashmi S, Vanderwert RE, Price HA, Gerson SA. Exploring the benefits of doll play through neuroscience. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020;14:413. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.560176
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.56017...
),(8Taner Derman M, Şahin Zeteroğlu E, Ergişi Birgül A. The effect of play-based math activities on different areas of development in children 48 to 60 months of age. SAGE Open. 2020;10(2):215824402091953. DOI:10.1177/2158244020919531
https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244020919531...
), (9Ramírez-Granizo IA, Ubago-Jiménez JL, González-Valero G, Puertas-Molero P, San Román-Mata S. The effect of physical activity and the use of active video games: exergames in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17(12):4243. DOI: 10.3390/ijerph17124243
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124243...
),(10Bjorklund DF, Brown RD. Physical play and cognitive development: integrating activity, cognition, and education. Child. Dev. 1998;69(3):604. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06229.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998...
.

Active games and play that promote engagement in physical activity can be defined as a physical activity that helps with energy expenditure, is usually done in an open environment, promotes fun, and should be performed at any intensity11Truelove S, Vanderloo LM, Tucker P. Defining and measuring active play among young children: a systematic review. J. Phys. Act. Health. 2017;14(2):155-66. DOI: 10.1123/jpah.2016-0195
https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.2016-0195...
.

They are currently a basic content for the education of children and adolescents and can meet the level of physical activity recommended daily, of moderate to vigorous intensity, by the World Health Organization (WHO)12World Health Organization. Who guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour web annex Evidence Profiles* [Internet]. 2020[Acesso em03 maio 2023]. Disponível em:Disponível em:https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/336657/9789240015111- eng.pdf?utm_medium=email&utm_source=transaction
https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/hand...
. Moreover, whether through the playground in the school environment, whether in school physical education or in interventions, they can be important to raise children’s physical activity levels and positively impact their health as a whole13Cardon G, Labarque V, Smits D, Bourdeaudhuij ID. Promoting physical activity at the pre-school playground: the effects of providing markings and play equipment. Prev. Med. 2009;48(4):335-40. DOI: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.02.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.02....
.

Silva and collaborators14Silva HVA da, Nascimento TA do, Lima TM de, Costa A dos S. The effect of an initiation to struggles structured program on the physical capacities, visual attention and school performance in elementary school children. Rev. Bras. Ciênc. Esporte. 2019;41(2):176-82. DOI: 10.1016/j.rbce.2018.04.012
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rbce.2018.04.0...
observed that opposition games and play of moderate to vigorous intensity were able to benefit children’s visual attention and academic performance. Additionally, interventions that can favor a more active school environment and reduce sedentary behavior through physical activity (games, breaks, sports, motor activities) can have a positive impact on cognitive functions (such as memory, attention, and motivation) and enhance the development of complex neural circuits and synaptic connections15Doherty A, Forés Miravalles A. Physical activity and cognition: inseparable in the classroom. Front. educ. 2019;4(4):105. DOI: 10.3389/feduc.2019.00105
https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2019.00105...
.

Some neurophysiological hypotheses have been postulated to explain possible changes in physical activity and its intensity (from moderate to vigorous), so we can speculate that active games and play: 1) increase cerebral blood flow; 2) boost the neurogenesis process 3) boost the angiogenesis process; 4) bring about a greater expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF); 5) promote a greater electrical activation of the cerebral cortex; 6) promote a greater activation of neurotransmitters; 7) improve the brain structure16Carlos A, Abujabra M Filho , Robles C, Alves R, Sepúlveda C, et al. Influência do exercício físico na cognição: uma atualização sobre mecanismos fisiológicos. Rev. Bras. Med. Esporte. 2014;20(3). DOI: 10.1590/1517-86922014200301930
https://doi.org/10.1590/1517-86922014200...
),(17Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson KI, Kienzler C, Drollette ES, Raine LB, Kao SC, et al. Physical activity increases white matter microstructure in children. Front. Neurosci. 2018;12:950. DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00950
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00950...
.

Notwithstanding findings on the benefits of games and play for children, to the best of our knowledge, there is no systematic review on the influence of active games and play on children’s cognition, taking into account observational and experimental studies. In this sense, the main purpose of this study is to synthesize the scientific literature about the influence of active games and play on children’s cognition.

Methods

Search and selection strategies

This systematic review was carried out following the guidelines of The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA)18Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. Updating guidance for reporting systematic reviews: development of the PRISMA 2020 statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2021;134:103-12. DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.02.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021....
. The review was registered and published in PROSPERO (CRD42021239900).

The databases used in this review were: Lilacs, PsycINFO, Pubmed, Scielo, and Scopus. The terms used in the search were structured from the following blocks: 1st block (play, “play and game”, “play and playthings”, “active play”, “pretend play”, “free play”, “risky play”, “outdoor play”, “creative play”, “unstructured play”, “outdoor time”); 2nd block (children, schoolchild, infant, kid, toddler, youngster); 3rd block (cognition, “cognitive function”, “executive function”, attention, memory, motivation). The article search strategy did not have a date established as a parameter for including articles, and the process ended on April 25, 2021.

The search strategy was established in blocks, using the Boolean operator “OR” in order to separate the terms and their respective synonyms. To join all blocks, the Boolean operator “AND” was used. No filter was applied during the search process in the databases. The articles found in the databases should contain the descriptors at least in the title, abstract or keywords so that the material could be integrated into the review for the selection stage. The entire article search, selection and analysis process was conducted by peers, independently, without contact or consultation between researchers. At the end of each stage of the verification process, divergences regarding the inclusion or exclusion of articles were resolved by a third researcher. Articles identified from the references and that met the eligibility criteria were included in the review.

The first stage of the selection was the search in the databases. After this process, data relating to each database was stored in the Endnote x9 program. Subsequently, duplicate studies were checked, and then the two authors read the titles and abstracts of the articles in order to verify whether they met the inclusion criteria. In case of disagreement between the two authors over the selection of a study, a third author would resolve the conflict. After approval of the articles in accordance with the inclusion criteria, they were read in full. For excluded articles, the reason was stated, based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria mentioned above.

Eligibility criteria

The following inclusion criteria were adopted: all studies that investigated the association and/or effect of active games and play on children’s cognitive functions; studies that used active games and/or play as one of their intervention strategies; quantitative studies with observational and experimental designs; studies with humans aged 3-12 years old; studies that fitted into the PICOS (Population, Intervention, Comparator, Outcomes and Study Design) strategy. The exclusion criteria were: studies that investigated the influence of electronic games on cognition; games and play that were not characterized as physical activity; books, review articles, dissertations and theses; studies that did not assess cognitive functions; studies that did not address games and play as an intervention strategy.

After the steps already mentioned, information about the year of publication, study location, and children’s characteristics (age, sex, number of participants) was extracted and recorded. As for the characteristics of the intervention/exposure, the type (acute or chronic), length, activities and intensity of active games and play were extracted. Additionally, the variables and objective measures of cognitive functions were identified. On the results of the studies included in the review, the p value and effect size were obtained. Finally, the methodological quality of the articles included in the review was analyzed. All stages, as well as the construction of the flowchart, and the analyses during the review were carried out in accordance with the PRISMA-P18Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. Updating guidance for reporting systematic reviews: development of the PRISMA 2020 statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2021;134:103-12. DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.02.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021....
standard.

Quality assessment

The Revised Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for randomized trials (RoB 2) was used to assess the methodological quality of the studies19Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, Elbers RG, Blencowe NS, Boutron I, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2019;366(1):l4898. DOI: 10.1136/bmj.l4898
https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l4898...
. RoB 2 is intended to assess the risk of bias in randomized trials through five domains (1 - bias from the randomization process; 2 - bias due to deviations from intended interventions; 3 - bias from absent results data; 4 - bias in result measuring; 5 - bias in selection of reported results). Each RoB 2 domain has signaling questions responsible for judging the risk of bias of the analyzed study. These are objective questions, and the answer options are: (1) Yes; (2) Probably yes; (3) Probably not; (4) No; (5) No information. This process was conducted by peers, and if there was any disagreement between the two authors, the third author would be consulted.

Results

In the present study, the search in the databases returned 3,812 articles. After duplicate articles were checked, taking into account title, author and year of publication, 844 duplicate studies were excluded. Thus, 2,968 were selected to have their title and abstract read, with 2,927 studies being excluded for not meeting the PICOS strategy and the inclusion criteria of the review. For the full reading stage, 41 articles were included. After reading and analysis following the eligibility criteria, 37 studies were excluded, with only four remaining. Finally, one article was included from the references, totaling five studies included in the review and quality analysis (Figure 1). Among these studies, three had a Randomized Crossover Clinical Trial (RCCT) design, all verifying the effect of games and play acutely on cognitive parameters20Efraim M, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, Tucker LA, Kwon S, Bailey BW. Acute after-school screen time in children decreases impulse control and activation toward high-calorie food stimuli in brain regions related to reward and attention. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;15(1):177-89. DOI: 10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00244...
),(21Tandon PS, Klein M, Saelens BE, Christakis DA, Marchese AJ, Lengua L. Short term impact of physical activity vs. sedentary behavior on preschoolers’ cognitive functions. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2018;15:17DOI: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.0...
),(22Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.14...
. The other two studies had a Randomized Clinical Trial (RCT) design, verifying the effect chronically. It is worth noting that no study with an observational design met the eligibility criteria of the review.

Figure 1
Flowchart of steps, according to PRISMA

Regarding the characteristics of the participants, all included studies were carried out with children aged between 4 and 12 years old, of both sexes. However, only in the study by Mezghanni et al.22Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.14...
, the sex of the participants was not mentioned.

The total number of participants in the studies included in that review ranged from 27 to 460, with highlight to the studies by Sánchez-López et al.23 and Pesce et al.24, which presented a larger sample than the other studies due to both having adopted a cluster randomized clinical trial design.

All articles included in that review have a randomized clinical trial design, as shown in Chart 1, with the purpose of investigating the effect of games and play on the central nervous system. In this sense, with regard to the characteristics of the exposure, two studies presented the length/time of exposure in a chronic manner, in the cluster format23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
),(24Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:349. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349...
, and three had an acute characteristic20Efraim M, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, Tucker LA, Kwon S, Bailey BW. Acute after-school screen time in children decreases impulse control and activation toward high-calorie food stimuli in brain regions related to reward and attention. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;15(1):177-89. DOI: 10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00244...
),(21Tandon PS, Klein M, Saelens BE, Christakis DA, Marchese AJ, Lengua L. Short term impact of physical activity vs. sedentary behavior on preschoolers’ cognitive functions. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2018;15:17DOI: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.0...
),(22Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.14...
.

Chart 1
Methodological information from the studies included in the review and that investigated the influence of active games and play on children’s cognition.

Sánchez-López et al.23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
carried out a multicomponent exposure containing three combined strategies: 1) traditional mid-class break games, sports games, games for developing motor skills, activities with music; 2) health promotion using pamphlets, information and guides for parents and teachers of the students in the study; 3) changes to the mid-class break environment with boards, circuits and equipment to promote a more physically active place. The exposure was held during one academic year. The experiences of games and play took place three times a week, lasting 60 minutes, with moderate to vigorous intensity. Pesce et al.24Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:349. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349...
conducted an intervention program lasting 1 hour per week for 6 months. The activities were proposed through cognitively enriched school physical education based on deliberate games of moderate to vigorous intensity.

About the studies that investigated the effect of games and play acutely on children’s cognition, Efraim et al.20Efraim M, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, Tucker LA, Kwon S, Bailey BW. Acute after-school screen time in children decreases impulse control and activation toward high-calorie food stimuli in brain regions related to reward and attention. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;15(1):177-89. DOI: 10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00244...
used 3 hours of exposure to various physically active games such as: bean bag toss, basketball, football, volleyball, walking on the track, Pokémon Go, fitness test. There was no mention of an intensity assessment during the intervention. Another acute study investigated the effects of active games and play during mid-class breaks and their impacts throughout the day on children’s cognition. The activities performed were hunting game and fishing game; the exact time of the mid-class break/exposure was not reported, and the intensity of the activities was not assessed22Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.14...
.

Tandon et al.21Tandon PS, Klein M, Saelens BE, Christakis DA, Marchese AJ, Lengua L. Short term impact of physical activity vs. sedentary behavior on preschoolers’ cognitive functions. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2018;15:17DOI: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.0...
used 15 minutes of aerobic physical activity as an intervention strategy, with games and play - more specifically, chase games, running, and jumping activities. The intensity of the activities was monitored by an accelerometer, with moderate to vigorous intensity being predominantly observed.

All included studies investigated the effect of games and play on some cognitive parameter in the target audience, as shown in Table 1. The studies by Tandon et al.21 and Pesce et al.24Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:349. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349...
presented the effects of the comparison group on inhibitory control. Pesce et al.24Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:349. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349...
verified the effect of ball skills on level of motor development, and how this change can impact inhibitory control.

It was found that three studies presented the effect of the intervention strategy from games and play on children’s cognition20Efraim M, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, Tucker LA, Kwon S, Bailey BW. Acute after-school screen time in children decreases impulse control and activation toward high-calorie food stimuli in brain regions related to reward and attention. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;15(1):177-89. DOI: 10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00244...
),(22Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.14...
),(23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
. Efraim et al.20Efraim M, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, Tucker LA, Kwon S, Bailey BW. Acute after-school screen time in children decreases impulse control and activation toward high-calorie food stimuli in brain regions related to reward and attention. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;15(1):177-89. DOI: 10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00244...
observed that the group that experienced active games showed significantly better performance for inhibitory control compared to the control group. Sánchez‐López et al.23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
, in their turn, observed that the multicomponent intervention, in a chronic form, with active games and play as one of the main intervention strategies, was able to generate an effect in all assessments of cognitive performance, referring to the pre x post condition, compared to the control group. Finally, Mezghanni et al.22Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.14...
found that games and play during mid-class breaks had a positive impact on children’s focused attention and reaction time throughout the day.

Table 1
Information on the results of the studies included in the review and that investigated the influence of active games and play on children’s cognition.

A quality analysis of the studies included in this review was performed using RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomized trials (Figure 2). Regarding the “randomization process” analysis criterion, all studies raised “some concerns” as to their risk of bias, since no study reported whether the allocation process was concealed. Furthermore, the study by Sánchez-López et al.23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
showed many differences in relation to baseline data before the intervention.

As for the “deviations from intended interventions” criterion, all studies presented “low risk of bias”, except for that by Sánchez-López et al.23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
, which was classified as raising “some concerns” due to participants and their caregivers knowing their attribution in relation to the exposures in the study. However, one of the authors’ explanations was that they used the strategy of the intervention activities being previously chosen by students/participants in order to increase compliance with the proposed activities. All studies were classified as “low risk of bias” in relation to the “absent results data” and “results measuring” criteria.

Finally, with regard to the “selection of reported results” criterion, one study presented “low risk of bias”20Efraim M, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, Tucker LA, Kwon S, Bailey BW. Acute after-school screen time in children decreases impulse control and activation toward high-calorie food stimuli in brain regions related to reward and attention. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;15(1):177-89. DOI: 10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00244...
, and one study was indicated as raising “some concerns” due to not presenting the data/results from both periods of the RCCT design22Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.14...
. Still on this criterion, three studies were classified as “high risk of bias”, mainly because they ran multiple statistical analyses that were not part of the study’s preliminary strategies23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
),(24Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:349. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349...
, as well as due to multiple measurement analyses (various assessment instruments) for a single variable21Tandon PS, Klein M, Saelens BE, Christakis DA, Marchese AJ, Lengua L. Short term impact of physical activity vs. sedentary behavior on preschoolers’ cognitive functions. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2018;15:17DOI: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.0...
.

Figure 2
Study quality analysis

Discussion

The purpose of this systematic review was to synthesize articles in the literature that investigated the effects, correlations and associations of active games and play on children’s cognition. To date, no systematic review has summarized the influence of active games and play, as well as their outcomes, on a child’s brain, taking into account observational and experimental studies.

In the present study, two articles reported results opposing the hypotheses found in the literature. Engagement in games and play, from the perspective of promoting physical activity, did not generate an effect on inhibitory control, but a comparison between conditions (sedentary or normal routine activity)21Tandon PS, Klein M, Saelens BE, Christakis DA, Marchese AJ, Lengua L. Short term impact of physical activity vs. sedentary behavior on preschoolers’ cognitive functions. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2018;15:17DOI: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.0...
),(24Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:349. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349...
did.

Tandon et al.21Tandon PS, Klein M, Saelens BE, Christakis DA, Marchese AJ, Lengua L. Short term impact of physical activity vs. sedentary behavior on preschoolers’ cognitive functions. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2018;15:17DOI: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.0...
and Pesce et al.24Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:349. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349...
, because interventions with active games and play do not have effects on cognition, brought possible explanations for the results obtained, such as: the chosen measuring instrument; transition between conditions (intervention and control) for the assessments; sample size; differences in baseline characteristics; non- blinding in the allocation process and non-blinding of the teachers involved in the intervention. On the other hand, different studies have indicated other possible explanations for the lack of effect of physical activity on a child’s brain, such as: difference from teacher to teacher when carrying out the interventions; students’ difficulty raising their intelligence scores; and low level of cognitive stimuli arising from the intervention activity25Resaland GK, Aadland E, Moe VF, Aadland KN, Skrede T, Stavnsbo M, et al. Effects of physical activity on schoolchildren’s academic performance: the active smarter kids (ASK) cluster-randomized controlled trial. Prev. Med. 2016;91:322-8. DOI: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.09.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.09....
),(26Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333(6045):959-64. DOI: 10.1126/science.1204529
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204529...
. However, three articles indicated that active games and play have positive effects on children’s cognitive parameters (Figure 3)20Efraim M, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, Tucker LA, Kwon S, Bailey BW. Acute after-school screen time in children decreases impulse control and activation toward high-calorie food stimuli in brain regions related to reward and attention. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;15(1):177-89. DOI: 10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00244...
),(22Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.14...
),(23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
.

Figure 3
Effects of games and play on children’s cognition

Regarding the positive effects of active games and play summarized in this systematic review, the study by Mezghanni et al.22Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.14...
highlights as possible explanations that activities developed during mid-class breaks play an important role in modulating children’s brains, with the following highlights: cognitive reorientation due to “active break/interval”; increased cerebral blood flow; greater activation of neurotransmitters and learning resulting from the activities experienced. Indeed, the study by Egger et al.27Egger F, Benzing V, Conzelmann A, Schmidt M. Boost your brain, while having a break! The effects of long-term cognitively engaging physical activity breaks on children’s executive functions and academic achievement. Parmenter B, editor. PLOS ONE. 2019;14(3):e0212482. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212482
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.021...
corroborates these findings, as they reported that recess and breaks during classes with

physical activity, and using games as a strategy, can contribute to improving the cognitive functions of school-aged children.

Following these assumptions, physical activity done in the classroom or during the school routine, according to a recent systematic review with meta-analysis, can have a positive impact on academic performance, but further research is needed to provide a more solid base to these results28Watson A, Timperio A, Brown H, Best K, Hesketh KD. Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on academic and physical activity outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017;14(1):1-24. DOI: 10.1186/s12966-017-0569-9
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0569-...
.

Engagement in physical activity through games and play or multicomponent intervention programs, such as the study by Sánchez-López et al.23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
, can promote adaptations that impact brain regions such as the prefrontal cortex and cerebellum, and positively influence children’s executive functions (inhibitory control, logical reasoning, verbal factor, numerical factor, spatial factor, and general intelligence). Thus, these changes in brain regions can lead to improved cognitive performance29Drollette ES, Scudder MR, Raine LB, Moore RD, Saliba BJ, Pontifex MB, et al. Acute exercise facilitates brain function and cognition in children who need it most: An ERP study of individual differences in inhibitory control capacity. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014Jan 1;7:53-64. DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.11.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.11.00...
. Furthermore, one of the hypotheses suggested is the possibility of helping increase BDNF expression and, consequently, stimulating the neuronal production process30Rodriguez-Ayllon M, Derks I, van den Dries M, Esteban Cornejo I, Labrecque JA, Yang-Huang J, et al. Associations of physical activity and screen time with white matter microstructure in children from the general population. SSRN Electronic Journal. 2018;205:116258. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116258
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.201...
. In addition to the neurobiological factor, the characteristics of playing and the stimuli that this activity can provide (ability to solve problems, management of emotions, cooperation, visuospatial stimuli) are capable of generating positive adaptations for the central nervous system26Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333(6045):959-64. DOI: 10.1126/science.1204529
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204529...
.

Efraim et al.20Efraim M, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, Tucker LA, Kwon S, Bailey BW. Acute after-school screen time in children decreases impulse control and activation toward high-calorie food stimuli in brain regions related to reward and attention. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;15(1):177-89. DOI: 10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00244...
used a different approach from the other articles included in this review by investigating brain implications from increased sedentary play versus active play after school on neural measures of eating control and inhibitory control. One of the outcomes presented was that the sedentary playing condition showed lower executive function performance (inhibitory control) compared to the active playing condition. In addition, the sedentary playing condition may make children more prone to resorting to foods with higher calorie content. In this sense, providing an active environment by increasing the availability of physical activities resulting from games, sports and aerobic activities can generate beneficial effects on executive functions31Sjöwall D, Hertz M, Klingberg T. No long-term effect of physical activity intervention on working memory or arithmetic in preadolescents. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1342. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01342
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01342...
.

Positive changes in executive functions, such as inhibitory control, selective attention, and working memory are fundamental to the daily lives of children, helping them with daily tasks, and very present in the school environment32Blair C. Educating executive function. Wiley interdiscip. rev. Cogn. sci.. 2016;8(1-2):e1403. DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1403
https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1403...
),(33Sankalaite S, Huizinga M, Dewandeleer J, Xu C, de Vries N, Hens E, et al. Strengthening executive function and self-regulation through teacher-student interaction in preschool and primary school children: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 2021;12. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.718262
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.71826...
. These brain functions are directly linked to writing, reading, arithmetic calculations, social communication, learning, and academic performance34Jirout J, LoCasale-Crouch J, Turnbull K, Gu Y, Cubides M, Garzione S, et al. How lifestyle factors affect cognitive and executive function and the ability to learn in children. Nutrients. 2019;11(8). DOI: 10.3390/nu11081953
https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11081953...
.

In summary, the main claims about the effect of active games and play on children’s brains are related to the characteristics and stimuli that the activities can provide, as well as to possible neurobiological adaptations. The motor/muscular activity resulting from games and play can promote signaling to various brain regions such as the motor cortex, prefrontal cortex, limbic system, and hippocampus. From this, they can stimulate attention, executive functions, emotions, and the reward system, and, consequently, contribute to the development of the child’s brain35Shaheen S. How child’s play impacts executive function-related behaviors. Appl. neuropsychol. Child. 2014Jul;3(3):182-7. DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2013.839612
https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2013.83...
.

Another very important parameter directly related to effects and adaptations as to children’s cognitive functions is the intensity of the activities performed. Several studies and reviews point out that physical activities of moderate to vigorous intensity are crucial to generating positive changes in children’s cognition36Tomporowski PD, Davis CL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Exercise and children’s intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Educ. psychol. rev. 2007;20(2):111-31. DOI: 10.1007/s10648-007-9057-0
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-007-9057-...
),(37Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM, Khan NA, Raine LB, Scudder MR, et al. Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics. 2014;134(4):e1063-71. DOI: 10.1542/peds.2013-3219
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-3219...
. Physical activity interventions of moderate to vigorous intensity can induce the production of Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor (VEGF) and thus boost the angiogenesis process, that is, increase cerebral vascularization. contributing to greater blood and oxygen supply to the brain38Morland C, Andersson KA, Haugen ØP, Hadzic A, Kleppa L, Gille A, et al. Exercise induces cerebral VEGF and angiogenesis via the lactate receptor HCAR1. Nature Communications. 2017;8(1): 15557. DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15557
https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms15557...
. Moreover, it promotes the action of Insulin-Like Growth Factor 1 (IGF-1), which, in its turn, signals and raises the amount of BDNF in the brain, bringing about an increase in the production of neurons39Van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat. Neurosci. 1999;2(3):266-70. DOI: 10.1038/6368
https://doi.org/10.1038/6368...
),(40Horowitz AM, Fan X, Bieri G, Smith LK, Sanchez-Diaz CI, Schroer AB, et al. Blood factors transfer beneficial effects of exercise on neurogenesis and cognition to the aged brain. Science. 2020;369(6500):167-73. DOI: 10.1126/science.aaw2622
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaw2622...
.

In this review, only the studies by Sánchez‐López et al.23Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383...
, Tandon et al.21Tandon PS, Klein M, Saelens BE, Christakis DA, Marchese AJ, Lengua L. Short term impact of physical activity vs. sedentary behavior on preschoolers’ cognitive functions. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2018;15:17DOI: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.0...
, and Pesce et al.24Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:349. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349...
determined or monitored the intensity of the games and play carried out. Considering this parameter is fundamental and can contribute to a better understanding of the possible effects of these activities, as well as to more effective intervention programs with regard to favorable changes to a child’s brain.

Although the results found are very interesting for this scientific field, in our systematic review study we can observe some limitations: 1) inclusion of only five databases in the article search process; 2) small number of articles included in the review;

3) non-inclusion of articles published in another language. However, this was the first synthesis of the literature through a systematic review on the influence of active games and play on children’s cognitive functions. Another important point of this review was the identification of the effect size of the included articles, enabling a greater breadth of the practical significance of the results, concomitantly with the analysis of the methodological quality level.

Despite the small number of studies found, the results reaffirm the importance of active games and play for children’s cognition. From a practical point of view, it is interesting to increase the time made available for this activity in the school environment, in combination with intervention programs outside school hours. Moreover, public policies aimed at offering more spaces and opportunities for children to experience active games and play are of utmost importance to increase their time of exposure to these activities.

Regarding the assessment of the methodological quality of the included studies, a greater methodological caution with respect to the participants allocation process and a greater concern with the data analysis strategy are essential. Another important point concerns assessments of cognitive function; it is necessary to avoid using multiple tests for a single cognitive measure.

However, it is important to be cautious when extrapolating these findings due to little evidence on the influence of active games and play on children’s brain. We highlight that further research is needed, with an experimental, cross-sectional, longitudinal and correlational design, to support this field of science.

Conclusion

Based on the synthesis carried out in this review, our findings elucidate that active games and play can have effects on children’s cognition. Of the five studies selected, three of them indicated positive effects of games and play on focused attention, reaction time, and executive functions (inhibitory control, logical reasoning, verbal factor, numerical factor, spatial factor, and general intelligence).

However, despite having investigated physically active games and play, those studies differ in terms of length, exposure time, assessment instruments, and interventions applied. Understanding these parameters and controlling the intensity of activities is fundamental to filling the current gaps in the literature on the subject. Nevertheless, and based on this review, we can identify experimental evidence in the literature about the effects of active games and play on cognition. Therefore, encouraging this sort of experience can be fundamental for cognitive development in childhood.

Acknowledgments

To the Graduate Program in Physical Education of the Federal University of Pernambuco (UFPE). To the Research Group on Physical Exercise, Nutrition and Central Nervous System (GENSC). To the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPQ).

References

  • Nijhof SL, Vinkers CH, van Geelen SM, Duijff SN, Achterberg EJM, van der Net J, et al. Healthy play, better coping: the importance of play for the development of children in health and disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018; 95(95):421-9. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.024
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.024
  • Lämmle C, Kobel S, Wartha O, Wirt T, Steinacker JM. Intervention effects of a school-based health promotion program on children’s motor skills. J. Public Health. 2016;24(3):185-92. DOI: 10.1007/s10389-016-0715-x
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10389-016-0715-x
  • Van der Fels IMJ, Te Wierike SCM, Hartman E, Elferink-Gemser MT, Smith J, Visscher C. The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4-16 year old typically developing children: a systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2015;18(6):697-703. DOI: 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.09.007
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2014.09.007
  • Cordazzo STD, Vieira ML. A brincadeira e suas implicações nos processos de aprendizagem e de desenvolvimento. Estud. Pesqui. Psicol. [Internet]. 2007[Acesso em 04 fev 2023];7(1). Disponível em: Disponível em: http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1808-42812007000100009.
    » http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1808-42812007000100009.
  • Wallon H. Do ato ao pensamento: ensaio de psicologia comparada. 2nd ed. Vol. 2. 2008.
  • Gray PH. The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and adolescents. Am. J. Play [Internet]. 2011Jan 1 [Acesso em 04 fev 2023];3(4):443-63. Disponível em:Disponível em:https://wowgym.bg/wp- content/uploads/2014/03/3-4-article-gray-decline-of-play.pdf
    » https://wowgym.bg/wp- content/uploads/2014/03/3-4-article-gray-decline-of-play.pdf
  • Hashmi S, Vanderwert RE, Price HA, Gerson SA. Exploring the benefits of doll play through neuroscience. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020;14:413. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.560176
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.560176
  • Taner Derman M, Şahin Zeteroğlu E, Ergişi Birgül A. The effect of play-based math activities on different areas of development in children 48 to 60 months of age. SAGE Open. 2020;10(2):215824402091953. DOI:10.1177/2158244020919531
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244020919531
  • Ramírez-Granizo IA, Ubago-Jiménez JL, González-Valero G, Puertas-Molero P, San Román-Mata S. The effect of physical activity and the use of active video games: exergames in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17(12):4243. DOI: 10.3390/ijerph17124243
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124243
  • Bjorklund DF, Brown RD. Physical play and cognitive development: integrating activity, cognition, and education. Child. Dev. 1998;69(3):604. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06229.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06229.x
  • Truelove S, Vanderloo LM, Tucker P. Defining and measuring active play among young children: a systematic review. J. Phys. Act. Health. 2017;14(2):155-66. DOI: 10.1123/jpah.2016-0195
    » https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.2016-0195
  • World Health Organization. Who guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour web annex Evidence Profiles* [Internet]. 2020[Acesso em03 maio 2023]. Disponível em:Disponível em:https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/336657/9789240015111- eng.pdf?utm_medium=email&utm_source=transaction
    » https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/336657/9789240015111- eng.pdf?utm_medium=email&utm_source=transaction
  • Cardon G, Labarque V, Smits D, Bourdeaudhuij ID. Promoting physical activity at the pre-school playground: the effects of providing markings and play equipment. Prev. Med. 2009;48(4):335-40. DOI: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.02.013
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.02.013
  • Silva HVA da, Nascimento TA do, Lima TM de, Costa A dos S. The effect of an initiation to struggles structured program on the physical capacities, visual attention and school performance in elementary school children. Rev. Bras. Ciênc. Esporte. 2019;41(2):176-82. DOI: 10.1016/j.rbce.2018.04.012
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rbce.2018.04.012
  • Doherty A, Forés Miravalles A. Physical activity and cognition: inseparable in the classroom. Front. educ. 2019;4(4):105. DOI: 10.3389/feduc.2019.00105
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2019.00105
  • Carlos A, Abujabra M Filho , Robles C, Alves R, Sepúlveda C, et al. Influência do exercício físico na cognição: uma atualização sobre mecanismos fisiológicos. Rev. Bras. Med. Esporte. 2014;20(3). DOI: 10.1590/1517-86922014200301930
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/1517-86922014200301930
  • Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson KI, Kienzler C, Drollette ES, Raine LB, Kao SC, et al. Physical activity increases white matter microstructure in children. Front. Neurosci. 2018;12:950. DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00950
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00950
  • Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. Updating guidance for reporting systematic reviews: development of the PRISMA 2020 statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2021;134:103-12. DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.02.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.02.003
  • Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, Elbers RG, Blencowe NS, Boutron I, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2019;366(1):l4898. DOI: 10.1136/bmj.l4898
    » https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l4898
  • Efraim M, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, Tucker LA, Kwon S, Bailey BW. Acute after-school screen time in children decreases impulse control and activation toward high-calorie food stimuli in brain regions related to reward and attention. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;15(1):177-89. DOI: 10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00244-y
  • Tandon PS, Klein M, Saelens BE, Christakis DA, Marchese AJ, Lengua L. Short term impact of physical activity vs. sedentary behavior on preschoolers’ cognitive functions. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2018;15:17DOI: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.004
  • Mezghanni N, Masmoudi L, Abdallah SB. Effect of play at recess on diurnal fluctuations in ability to refocus cognitively in pupils aged 11-12 years. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2018;50(5):758-68. DOI: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2018.1498195
  • Sánchez‐López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. 2019;29(5):766-75. DOI: 10.1111/sms.13383
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13383
  • Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:349. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349
  • Resaland GK, Aadland E, Moe VF, Aadland KN, Skrede T, Stavnsbo M, et al. Effects of physical activity on schoolchildren’s academic performance: the active smarter kids (ASK) cluster-randomized controlled trial. Prev. Med. 2016;91:322-8. DOI: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.09.005
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.09.005
  • Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333(6045):959-64. DOI: 10.1126/science.1204529
    » https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204529
  • Egger F, Benzing V, Conzelmann A, Schmidt M. Boost your brain, while having a break! The effects of long-term cognitively engaging physical activity breaks on children’s executive functions and academic achievement. Parmenter B, editor. PLOS ONE. 2019;14(3):e0212482. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212482
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212482
  • Watson A, Timperio A, Brown H, Best K, Hesketh KD. Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on academic and physical activity outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017;14(1):1-24. DOI: 10.1186/s12966-017-0569-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0569-9
  • Drollette ES, Scudder MR, Raine LB, Moore RD, Saliba BJ, Pontifex MB, et al. Acute exercise facilitates brain function and cognition in children who need it most: An ERP study of individual differences in inhibitory control capacity. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014Jan 1;7:53-64. DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.11.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.11.001
  • Rodriguez-Ayllon M, Derks I, van den Dries M, Esteban Cornejo I, Labrecque JA, Yang-Huang J, et al. Associations of physical activity and screen time with white matter microstructure in children from the general population. SSRN Electronic Journal. 2018;205:116258. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116258
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116258
  • Sjöwall D, Hertz M, Klingberg T. No long-term effect of physical activity intervention on working memory or arithmetic in preadolescents. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1342. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01342
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01342
  • Blair C. Educating executive function. Wiley interdiscip. rev. Cogn. sci.. 2016;8(1-2):e1403. DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1403
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1403
  • Sankalaite S, Huizinga M, Dewandeleer J, Xu C, de Vries N, Hens E, et al. Strengthening executive function and self-regulation through teacher-student interaction in preschool and primary school children: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 2021;12. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.718262
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.718262
  • Jirout J, LoCasale-Crouch J, Turnbull K, Gu Y, Cubides M, Garzione S, et al. How lifestyle factors affect cognitive and executive function and the ability to learn in children. Nutrients. 2019;11(8). DOI: 10.3390/nu11081953
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11081953
  • Shaheen S. How child’s play impacts executive function-related behaviors. Appl. neuropsychol. Child. 2014Jul;3(3):182-7. DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2013.839612
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2013.839612
  • Tomporowski PD, Davis CL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Exercise and children’s intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Educ. psychol. rev. 2007;20(2):111-31. DOI: 10.1007/s10648-007-9057-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-007-9057-0
  • Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM, Khan NA, Raine LB, Scudder MR, et al. Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics. 2014;134(4):e1063-71. DOI: 10.1542/peds.2013-3219
    » https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-3219
  • Morland C, Andersson KA, Haugen ØP, Hadzic A, Kleppa L, Gille A, et al. Exercise induces cerebral VEGF and angiogenesis via the lactate receptor HCAR1. Nature Communications. 2017;8(1): 15557. DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15557
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms15557
  • Van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat. Neurosci. 1999;2(3):266-70. DOI: 10.1038/6368
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/6368
  • Horowitz AM, Fan X, Bieri G, Smith LK, Sanchez-Diaz CI, Schroer AB, et al. Blood factors transfer beneficial effects of exercise on neurogenesis and cognition to the aged brain. Science. 2020;369(6500):167-73. DOI: 10.1126/science.aaw2622
    » https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaw2622

Edited by

Editor: Carlos Herold Jr.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    11 Mar 2024
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    23 May 2023
  • Accepted
    11 Jan 2024
Universidade Estadual de Maringá Avenida Colombo, 5790 - cep: 87020-900 - tel: 44 3011 4315 - Maringá - PR - Brazil
E-mail: revdef@uem.br