Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Adaptation of the Child Development Clinical Risk Indicators instrument to retrospective parent report

Abstracts

PURPOSE:

To adapt the Child Development Clinical Risk Indicators (IRDI) instrument to retrospective report for parents of children from 3 to 7 years old and administer it in two groups.

METHODS:

Participated on the study 72 subjects - parents of children aged from 2 years and 11 months to 7 years and 7 months - divided into two groups: Research Group and Control Group. The proposed modification to parent report transformed the 31 indicators (affirmations) into retrospective self-administered questions to parents, with responses in Likert scale. The IRDI-questionnaire was administered in the two groups.

RESULTS:

The IRDI-questionnaire showed to be easy and quick to administer (average 15 minutes) and with low cost. The analysis of the questions obtained a good internal consistency value. The comparison between the groups by the parents' answers to the 31 questions showed difference in 16 questions.

CONCLUSION:

The items of the IRDI-questionnaire concern the main Autism Spectrum Disorders risk signs pointed out in literature. The parents' answers showed differences between the studied groups.

Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences; Autistic disorder; Questionnaires; Communication; Language


OBJETIVO:

Adaptar o instrumento Indicadores Clínicos de Risco para o Desenvolvimento Infantil (IRDI) para questionário retrospectivo para pais de crianças de 3 a 7 anos e aplicá-lo em dois grupos de sujeitos.

MÉTODOS:

Participaram do estudo 72 sujeitos - pais de crianças de 2 anos e 11 meses a 7 anos e 7 meses - divididos em dois grupos: Grupo Pesquisa e Grupo Controle. A proposta de modificação do instrumento para questionário transformou os 31 indicadores (afirmações) do primeiro em perguntas de caráter retrospectivo, dirigidas aos pais em formato autoaplicável, com respostas em escala Likert. O IRDI-questionário foi aplicado nos dois grupos estudados.

RESULTADOS:

O IRDI-questionário mostrou-se de fácil aplicação, com rapidez no preenchimento (tempo médio de 15 minutos) e baixo custo. A análise das questões que compõem o instrumento revelou boa consistência interna. A comparação entre os grupos, segundo as respostas dos pais às 31 questões do IRDI-questionário, mostrou diferença em 16 delas.

CONCLUSÃO:

Foi possível verificar que os itens do questionário recobrem os principais sinais de risco para Transtorno do Espectro do Autismo apontados na literatura. Além disso, as respostas dos pais assinalaram que houve diferença entre os grupos estudados.

Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences; Autistic disorder; Questionnaires; Communication; Language


INTRODUCTION

The infantile autism has been studied by various areas of knowledge since its first description in 1943 by Léo Kanner. Since then, its definition has undergone several modifications and, currently, the great variability in clinical signs shown by this group of individuals made the use of the term "autism spectrum disorders" (ASD) ​more appropriate( 11. Volkmar FR, Pauls D. Autism. Lancet. 2003;362(9390):1133-41. ).

It is known that language difficulties are an important dimension of this clinical condition, next to impairments in social interaction and behavior( 11. Volkmar FR, Pauls D. Autism. Lancet. 2003;362(9390):1133-41.

2. Thurm A, Lord C, Lee LC, Newschaffer C. Predictors of language acquisition in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1721-34.

3. Eigsti IM, Bennetto L, Dadlani MB. Beyond pragmatics: morphosyntactic development in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1007-23.
- 44. Weismer SE, Lord C, Esler A. Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:1259-73. ).

In general, the clinical signs of ASD can be observed in very young children, since the signs involve changes in skills that are typically developed in the first 2 years of life( 55. Matson JL, Wilkins J, Gonzalez M. Early identification and diagnosis in autism spectrum disorders in young children and infants: how early is too early? Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2008;2:75-84.

6. Wetherby AM, Woods J, Allen L, Cleary J, Dickinson H, Lord C. Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(5):473-93.

7. Dewrang P, Sandberg AD. Parental retrospective assessment of development and behavior in Asperger syndrome during the first 2 years of life. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010;4:461-73.
- 88. Horovitz M, Matson JL, Tuygin N, Beighley JS. The relationship between gender and age of first concern in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6:466-71. ).

In this context, some variability in the concerns expressed by parents occurs; however, complaints about the delay in the development of oral language are among the most common and consensual ones( 44. Weismer SE, Lord C, Esler A. Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:1259-73. , 99. Levy SE, Mandell DS, Schultz RT. Autism. Lancet. 2009; 374(9701):1627-38. , 1010. Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Lord C, Rogers S, Carter A, Carver L, et al. Clinical assessment and management of toddlers with suspected autism spectrum disorder: insights from studies of high-risk infants. Pediatrics. 2009;123:1383-91. ).

Family concerns in relation to the language development of these children is one of the reasons why the speech language pathologist is, in many cases, the first professional sought by the family, even before the diagnosis of the ASD has been advised or investigated. Thus, the relevance of speech language pathology and audiology in identifying possible risk for ASD is evident.

The significant results of intensive therapeutic interventions with very young children have encouraged this movement, which led to the need for use of diagnostic, assessment, and identification tools as a starting point for a differentiated prognosis( 55. Matson JL, Wilkins J, Gonzalez M. Early identification and diagnosis in autism spectrum disorders in young children and infants: how early is too early? Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2008;2:75-84. , 1111. Matson JL, Beighley J, Turygin N. Autism diagnosis and screening: factors to consider in differential diagnosis. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6:19-24.

12. Itzchak EB, Zachor DA. Who benefits from early intervention in autism spectrum disorders? Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2011;5:345-50.
- 1313. LoVullo SV, Matson JL. Development of a critical item algorithm for the baby and infant screen for children with autism traits. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6(1):378-84. ).

Articulating the academic perspective of research and procedures in Public Health, it is recommended to identify children who are at risk for an eventual diagnosis of ASD using effective and low-cost methods. The main strategy toward this purpose, according to several authors, would be the use of an instrument in the format of a retrospective questionnaire for parents( 1414. Reznick JS, Baranek GT, Watson LR, Crais ER. A parent-report instrument for identifying one-year-olds at risk for eventual diagnosis of autism: the first year inventory. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1691-710.

15. Rogers SJ, Hepburn S, Wehner E. Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2003;33(6):631-42.
- 1616. Clifford SM, Dissanayake C. The early development of joint attention in infants with autistic disorder using home video observations and parental interview. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:791-805. ).

In Brazil, it is still very common for children to get to speech language pathology and audiology therapy at around 3-4 years of age, with recurrent complaints of delay in language development, often with suspected hearing loss, without any prior research or diagnosis for ASD having been performed( 1717. Sousa EC, Lima FT, Tamanaha AC, Perissinoto J, Azevedo MF, Chiari BM. A associação entre a suspeita inicial de perda auditiva e a ausência de comunicação verbal em crianças com transtornos do espectro autístico. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(4):487-90. ).

In Brazil, the aim at the development of instruments for early detection of problems in child development, a multicentered study (in the period of 2000-2008), with the support of the Ministry of Health, the São Paulo Research Foundation and the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development, was carried out. From this study an instrument has emerged, which comprises the Clinical Risk Indicators for Child Development (Indicadores Clínicos de Risco para o Desenvolvimento Infantil, IRDI), consisting of 31 indicators observable in the first 18 months of the child's life, whose use is indicated for pediatricians and other health professionals( 1818. Kupfer MCM, Jerusalinsky AN, Bernardino LF, Wanderlet D, Rocha PSP, Molina SE, et al. Valor preditivo de indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil: um estudo a partir da teoria psicanalítica. Rev Latinoam Psicopatol Fundam. 2010;13(1):31-52. ). It is noteworthy that the IRDI is not a specific instrument for ASD, but for the problems in child development broadly.

This study aimed at articulating the important results of the research developed with IRDI in health to the interventions made ​​by Brazilian speech-language pathologists, who often meet - as evidenced by the clinical experience - with children showing signs of risk for ASD and having difficulty in accessing free-of-charge clinical tools in Brazil.

In this sense, it seems promising to investigate the applicability of the IRDI instrument in the form of a questionnaire for parents, which is the theme of this research.

Such considerations support what is proposed by this study, namely to adapt the IRDI instrument into a retrospective questionnaire for parents of children from 3 to 7 years of age and apply it, for the sake of comparison, in two groups of participants (affected and not-affected by ASD).

METHODS

This study was approved by the ethics committee in research of the university in which it was developed (protocol no. 117/2011).

Casuistry

The sample was obtained by convenience, with consecutive selection. A total of 72 individuals took part in the research: 65 mothers and 7 parents of children aged from 2 years and 11 months to 7 years and 7 months.

The subjects were divided into Study Group (SG) and Control Group (CG), to preserve the validity analysis and the reliability of the instrument.

The SG consisted of 34 mothers (94.4%) and 2 fathers (5.6%) of children who were selected according to the inclusion criteria described next, being 29 of them males (80.6%) and 7 of them females (19.4%), who received autism ambulatory care from the Institute of Psychiatry of the Hospital das Clínicas of the School of Medicine, Universidade de São Paulo (HC-FMUSP), and in private practices, aged between 35 and 91 months (2 years and 11 months and 7 years and 7 months).

Inclusion criteria were children with diagnosed ASD, according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition criteria(19) (American Psychiatric Association, 1995), and the absence of genetic, metabolic, and/or neurological disorders diagnosis, with the "IRDI questionnaire" answered by the parents, and the informed consent signed by them.

The CG consisted of 31 mothers (86.1%) and 5 fathers (13.9%) of children selected following the criteria described as follows: 23 male children (63.9%) and 13 female ones (36.1%), aged between 39 and 82 months (3 years and 3 months and 6 years and 10 months), with typical development and who attended the Public School of Early Childhood Education (EMEI) Curumim I, in Santana de Parnaíba.

Inclusion criteria were children without complaints, by the parents and by the school, regarding their development and the absence of genetic, neurologic, and/or metabolic disorders diagnosis, with the "IRDI questionnaire" answered by the parents and the informed consent signed by them.

Material

IRDI questionnaire: adapted IRDI into a retrospective questionnaire for parents with scaled answers.

Note: the IRDI instrument was validated in Brazil, consisting of 31 observable indicators in the first 18 months of the child's life. Its application is given by observation, followed by the recording of the results into three categories: present, absent, or not verified indicators. The absence of at least two indicators indicates risk of development( 1414. Reznick JS, Baranek GT, Watson LR, Crais ER. A parent-report instrument for identifying one-year-olds at risk for eventual diagnosis of autism: the first year inventory. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1691-710. ).

The adaptation of the IRDI instrument into the IRDI questionnaire turned its 31 indicators (statements) into questions (retrospective ones), directed toward parents, in a self-report format. The adaptation procedure is described in the next section.

Procedure

Adaptation of the instrument

The IRDI questionnaire is introduced by considerations about the nature of the questions and instructions regarding its filling out.

Then, information regarding both the informant and the child is requested.

The 31 statements of the original instrument (IRDI) were transformed into retrospective questions, directed toward parents. For this, only the verbal tense (from present to simple past) was modified, in order to preserve their original contents.

The answers, which in the original instrument were "absent," "present," and "unverified," in the new format (questionnaire) were placed on a Likert scale that quantified the behaviors into five categories: "never," "rarely," "sometimes," "often," and "always." In addition to these categories, the option "do not remember" was also included, because it was a retrospective questionnaire, and therefore, based solely on the memory of parents about the early development of their children. The Likert scale was the chosen one, consisting of categories that are linked in a continuum, so as not to unduly constrain the answers, in favor of the reliability of the instrument.

After the completion of this adaptation, it was given to two professionals, with extensive experience in administering the IRDI in its original format, to assess the IRDI questionnaire and suggest the necessary adjustments (which were not significant). Afterwards, the IRDI questionnaire was applied in both studied groups.

Administering the Clinical Risk Indicators for Child Development questionnaire in the Study Group

As part of the routine service of the Autism Clinic of the Institute of Psychiatry of the HC-USP, a team of researchers developed a protocol for multidisciplinary evaluations.

For the purposes of this study, patients who attended the service for routine psychiatric care were approached by the research team to check if they had interest in participating in the multidisciplinary assessments. Once confirmed about the interest of the parents, a return would be scheduled for the evaluations, including those relating to this research.

On the scheduled date, the researcher would provide ​​a brief explanation about the ethical issues contained in the informed consent and about the contents of IRDIquestionnaire, which was delivered to one of the parents (the one who volunteered so) so as to answer the questions. It is observed that the average time taken to fill out the questionnaire was 15 minutes.

Administering the Clinical Risk Indicators for Child Development questionnaire in the Control Group

After the paperwork needed for the authorization of the Education Department of the city of Santana de Parnaíba for carrying out of this research, the researcher attended the school, designated by the City Hall, in order to get in touch with the direction. It was agreed that the "IRDIquestionnaires" would be distributed by the researcher in PTA meetings, scheduled according to the school year's calendar.

After the selection of the classrooms where the questionnaires would be handed in (according to the age groups previously established as inclusion criteria for the study), it was verified with the direction of the EMEI if there were complaints regarding any students, as to their development both in general terms and specifically about language and/or behavior. Students with complaints of this nature from the school were excluded from the study.

On the date of the meeting with the parents, the researcher provided a brief explanation about the study and investigated, at the time the questionnaires were handed in, if the parents had any complaints as to the development of their children regarding their development in general terms, and in language and/or behavior, so as to exclude those with complaints.

An explanation about the contents of the IRDIquestionnaire, which was handed in to one of the parents (the one who would volunteer so) to be filled out by, was also performed. The average filling out time was 15 minutes.

Statistical analysis

A descriptive analysis of the data using absolute and relative frequencies, measures of central tendency (average and median), and dispersion (standard deviation (SD), minimum and maximum) was conducted. To assess the homogeneity between the groups (CG and SG), we used the association of the χ2 test and the comparison of the Student's t-test averages. In the internal consistency analysis of the IRDIquestionnaire, the Cronbach's a was used. Statistical significance was taken on a significance level of 5% (p≤0.05).

RESULTS

The IRDIquestionnaire proved itself to be easily applied, quick to be filled out (15 minutes on average), and to have low cost.

Up next, the results of its application in the two studied groups (SG and CG) are presented.

The studied population comprised 72 children; 36 in each group. It is observed that there was a higher percentage of boys in both groups and that this distribution was homogeneous (p=0.114; Table 1). The average age of the children in the CG was 58.8 months (SD=9.2) and, for the children of the SG, it was 62.6 months (SD=15.0), presenting no difference between the groups (p=0.199). As to the parental educational level, there was also no difference between the groups (p=0.690; Table 2).

Table 1
Number and percentual of children according to the study group, gender, and school education
Table 2
Description of age at the time of the diagnosis and time elapsed between the diagnosis and the administration of the questionnaire, only for the Study Group

Regarding the age at the time of the diagnosis and the time elapsed between diagnosis and the questionnaire's application in the SG, it appears that the average age of diagnosis is approximately 3 years and 6 months, ranging between 2 years and 5 months and 4 years and 9 months. In relation to the time elapsed, the average was 20 months (1 year and 8 months), with a minimum time less than 1 month and a maximum of 3 years and 10 months. It is noteworthy that the IRDIquestionnaire was administered to some children concurrently with the diagnostic process; therefore, the minimum time spent between diagnosis and completion of the questionnaire was less than a month in these cases.

After checking the homogeneity between the groups, the analysis of the internal consistency of the 31 questions of the instrument by Cronbach's α, which had a value of 0.89, was performed; α value ≥0.6 was considered as satisfactory for internal consistency. Thus, it can be stated that the IRDIquestionnaire presented good internal consistency( 2020. Sato FP. Validação da versão em português de um questionário para avaliação de autismo infantil [dissertação]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2008. ).

The comparison between the groups has shown differences in 16 questions, which can be seen in Table 3.

Table 3
Number and percentage of children according to group and questions of the Clinical Risk Indicators for Child Development questionnaire (1-31)

A higher proportion of children in the CG whose parents answered "always" to the question of whether there was an exchange of glances between the child and the mother, when compared to the answers of parents of children with ASD (p=0.006). According to parents, the likelihood of the child "always," when requesting their mother, wait for their mother's response was higher in the CG in relation to the SG (p=0.002).

It was also found that the CG children were more likely to react (smile, vocalize) when the mother or other person addressed them when compared to children with ASD (p<0.001). Children in the CG had a higher proportion in the reaction of "actively search for their mother's look" in relation to those with ASD (p<0.001). In the same way, the proportion of children who would "always" ask for help from someone else instead of remaining passive was higher in the CG (p=0.031).

In the CG, there is a higher proportion of children whose mothers "always" perceived that some requests from the child could be a way to draw their attention, when compared to the answers of parents of children with ASD (p<0.001).

The answers of the parents indicate that the probability of the child "always" actively seeking games and loving playfulness with their mother, during physical care procedures, was higher in the CG when compared to that in the SG (p=0.005).

According to parents, children in the CG are more likely to "always" show like or dislike toward something when compared to those in the SG (p=0.030). It is observed that in the CG the probability of mothers and children sharing a particular and private language between themselves was higher than that in the SG (p=0.023). It is also observed that the CG children are more likely to "make cute stunts" when compared to those of the SG (p<0.001). Likewise, from the parents' answers, it is noticeable that the CG children are more likely to get the look of approval from the adult when compare to those with ASD (p<0.001).

The parent responses indicate that 27.8% of the mothers of children with ASD "never" felt not obliged to fulfill everything the child would ask for, whereas 8.3% of the mothers of children in the CG felt the same (p=0.037).

In the answers of the parents, it is still possible to see that the probability of the child looking with curiosity toward what interests the mother was higher in the CG (p<0.001).

Therefore, in the comparison between the studied groups, through the IRDIquestionnaire, it was possible to distinguish children with ASD from the ones presenting typical development, considering the 16 (of the 31) questions that were statistically significant.

DISCUSSION

The importance of using screening and tracking tools for the early diagnosis of ASD is widely discussed in the literature( 1111. Matson JL, Beighley J, Turygin N. Autism diagnosis and screening: factors to consider in differential diagnosis. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6:19-24.

12. Itzchak EB, Zachor DA. Who benefits from early intervention in autism spectrum disorders? Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2011;5:345-50.

13. LoVullo SV, Matson JL. Development of a critical item algorithm for the baby and infant screen for children with autism traits. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6(1):378-84.

14. Reznick JS, Baranek GT, Watson LR, Crais ER. A parent-report instrument for identifying one-year-olds at risk for eventual diagnosis of autism: the first year inventory. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1691-710.

15. Rogers SJ, Hepburn S, Wehner E. Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2003;33(6):631-42.
- 1616. Clifford SM, Dissanayake C. The early development of joint attention in infants with autistic disorder using home video observations and parental interview. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:791-805. , 1818. Kupfer MCM, Jerusalinsky AN, Bernardino LF, Wanderlet D, Rocha PSP, Molina SE, et al. Valor preditivo de indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil: um estudo a partir da teoria psicanalítica. Rev Latinoam Psicopatol Fundam. 2010;13(1):31-52. , 2020. Sato FP. Validação da versão em português de um questionário para avaliação de autismo infantil [dissertação]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2008.

21. Pesaro ME. Alcance e limites teórico-metodológicos da pesquisa multicêntrica de indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2010.
- 2222. Lerner R. Indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil - IRDI: verificação da capacidade discriminativa entre autismo, retardo mental e normalidade [livre-docência]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2011. ). However, these instruments should have some basic features such as being easy to be applied, with simple questions and answers, being lowincost and being effective in the tracking which it is proposed to( 1414. Reznick JS, Baranek GT, Watson LR, Crais ER. A parent-report instrument for identifying one-year-olds at risk for eventual diagnosis of autism: the first year inventory. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1691-710. , 1919. American Psychiatric Association. Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais - DSM-IV-TR - Texto Revisado. Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas; 2002. ).

The use of the IRDIquestionnaire, in the sample studied, showed itself to be adequate in relation to the understanding of the questions and the pattern of answers given by the interviewees, as well as being quick and easy to be applied. The IRDI questionnaire showed good internal consistency. This result is consistent with other reliability studies of instruments used for the screening and diagnosis of ASD: Autism Screening Questionnaire, CARS-BR (the Brazilian version of the Childhood Autism Rating Scale) and Childhood Autism Rating Scale( 2020. Sato FP. Validação da versão em português de um questionário para avaliação de autismo infantil [dissertação]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2008. , 2323. Pereira A, Riesgo RS, Wagner MB. Autismo infantil: tradução e validação da Childhood Autism Rating Scale para uso no Brasil. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2008;84(6):487-94. , 2424. Shopler E, Reichler RJ, Devellis RF, Daly K. Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). J Autism Dev Disord. 1980;10:91-103. ).

The results show that many of the questions whose answers had statistical significance include important signs of risk for ASD, such as the exchange of looks, shared attention, and social interaction.

The matter of exchange of glances or looks between the child and the mother addresses a sign of ASD widely studied in the literature( 11. Volkmar FR, Pauls D. Autism. Lancet. 2003;362(9390):1133-41. , 55. Matson JL, Wilkins J, Gonzalez M. Early identification and diagnosis in autism spectrum disorders in young children and infants: how early is too early? Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2008;2:75-84. , 1313. LoVullo SV, Matson JL. Development of a critical item algorithm for the baby and infant screen for children with autism traits. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6(1):378-84. , 1414. Reznick JS, Baranek GT, Watson LR, Crais ER. A parent-report instrument for identifying one-year-olds at risk for eventual diagnosis of autism: the first year inventory. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1691-710. , 2525. Poon KK, Watson LR, Baranek GT, Poe MD. To what extent do joint attention, imitation, and object play behaviors in infancy predict later communication and intellectual functioning in ASD? J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(6):1064-74. ). Besides the IRDIquestionnaire, some other studies also deal with the subject of the gaze and shared attention in subsequent age groups.

In a study that examined the discriminative ability of the IRDl questionnaires among ASD, mental retardation, and normality by administering the instrument to family videos of children who were later on diagnosed with autism, the author found compatible results when verifying that the average chance of an autistic baby to show like or dislike toward something is 1.82 times higher than that of a normal baby( 2121. Pesaro ME. Alcance e limites teórico-metodológicos da pesquisa multicêntrica de indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2010. ).

Another study of family movies of children who were later on diagnosed with autism found that the social profile of an autistic baby differs from that of a normal baby when considering the average duration of glances addressed to the parents and the duration and frequency of social smiling( 2626. Brisson J, Serres J, Gattegno MP, Adrien JL. Étude des troubles précoces du contact social à partir de l'ánalyse des films familiaux chez des nourrissons de la naissance à 6 mois ultérieurement diagnostiqués autistes. Devenir. 2011;23(1):87-106. ).

The literature is vast and consensual when pointing out the deficits in social skills and in shared attention as strong early signs of ASD( 1616. Clifford SM, Dissanayake C. The early development of joint attention in infants with autistic disorder using home video observations and parental interview. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:791-805. , 2727. Ward S, Gilmore L. The autistic behavioural indicators instrument (ABII): development and instrument utility in discriminating autistic disorder from speech and language impairment and typical development. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010;4:28-42. ). Thus, the studies corroborate the results obtained here by claiming that, in children with ASD, the lack of interest in social stimuli is characterized by reduced frequency of eye contact; these children having a preference for non-social stimuli, manifested by directing the gaze preferentially to objects and not to individuals, along with the lack of interest in sharing attention( 1616. Clifford SM, Dissanayake C. The early development of joint attention in infants with autistic disorder using home video observations and parental interview. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:791-805. , 2727. Ward S, Gilmore L. The autistic behavioural indicators instrument (ABII): development and instrument utility in discriminating autistic disorder from speech and language impairment and typical development. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010;4:28-42.

28. Maestro S, Muratori F, Cesari A, Cavallaro M, Paziente A, Pecini E, et al. Course of autism signs in the first year of life. Psychopathology. 2005;38:26-31.
- 2929. Toth K, Munson J, Meltzoff AN, Dawson G. Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism spectrum disorder: joint attention, imitation, and toy play. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:993-1005. ).

An important concept introduced by Laznik et al.( 3030. Laznik MC, Maestro S, Muratori F, Parlato E. Interações sonoras entre bebês que se tornaram autistas e seus pais. In: Anais do Colóquio franco-brasileiro sobre a clínica com bebês. Paris; 2005 [cited 2014 Mar 21]. Available from: http://www.proceedings.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=MSC0000000072005000100004&lng=en&nrm=abn
Available from: htt...
) regarding the three stages of the instinctive cycle is discussed in Question 15 of the IRDIquestionnaire. According to the authors, the subjective constitution is given in three stages: the first stage, called active, is the one in which the baby goes in search of the external object (oral) in order to seize it. The second stage, the reflective and self-erotic one, is the one in which the child takes the object as a part of their own body (e.g., thumb/hand sucking). The third stage is the one in which the child is offered as the object of another subject (e.g., when the baby stretches their foot toward the mother's mouth and the two of them experience a shared pleasure in this game). According to the author, the autistic child does not reach the third stage of the cycle( 3030. Laznik MC, Maestro S, Muratori F, Parlato E. Interações sonoras entre bebês que se tornaram autistas e seus pais. In: Anais do Colóquio franco-brasileiro sobre a clínica com bebês. Paris; 2005 [cited 2014 Mar 21]. Available from: http://www.proceedings.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=MSC0000000072005000100004&lng=en&nrm=abn
Available from: htt...
). Without denying the effect of the organic factors involved in this clinical panorama, the failure in settling the third stage of the instinctive cycle suggests that the possibility of placing themselves in someone's place, when the baby offers themselves as an object to others, does not occur in the case of autistic children( 3030. Laznik MC, Maestro S, Muratori F, Parlato E. Interações sonoras entre bebês que se tornaram autistas e seus pais. In: Anais do Colóquio franco-brasileiro sobre a clínica com bebês. Paris; 2005 [cited 2014 Mar 21]. Available from: http://www.proceedings.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=MSC0000000072005000100004&lng=en&nrm=abn
Available from: htt...
).

The IRDIquestionnaire also contains some questions concerning the participation of the mother and her attitudes in relation to their child's behavior. The results of this study showed that two among these were noticeable after the statistical analysis conducted. To that extent, worth noting is the peculiarity introduced by the research that has elaborated the IRDI, including the relationship between mother and baby to the center of observation, differently from what predominantly occurs when it comes to signs of risk for ASD, focusing their observation solely on the child( 1818. Kupfer MCM, Jerusalinsky AN, Bernardino LF, Wanderlet D, Rocha PSP, Molina SE, et al. Valor preditivo de indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil: um estudo a partir da teoria psicanalítica. Rev Latinoam Psicopatol Fundam. 2010;13(1):31-52. , 2121. Pesaro ME. Alcance e limites teórico-metodológicos da pesquisa multicêntrica de indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2010. , 2222. Lerner R. Indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil - IRDI: verificação da capacidade discriminativa entre autismo, retardo mental e normalidade [livre-docência]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2011. ).

The results obtained here differ from those of the multicenter study conducted with the IRDI, which showed a group of 15 indicators with the ability to predict the risk for child development, out of which only 5 would match those indicated in this study, according to the adopted criterion for statistical significance. This might be because the present study focused on children with ASD, unlike what happened with the research mentioned, which has focused its attention on the problems of child development more broadly.

In the same vein, it is noteworthy that the issues that have the "motherese" theme have not been highlighted in the statistical analysis performed in this study. The studies in psycholinguistics of the prosody of mothers of children with autism show significant differences in the melodic curves analyzed - a fact that certainly served as base for the inclusion of items that address this aspect in the preparation of the IRDI( 3030. Laznik MC, Maestro S, Muratori F, Parlato E. Interações sonoras entre bebês que se tornaram autistas e seus pais. In: Anais do Colóquio franco-brasileiro sobre a clínica com bebês. Paris; 2005 [cited 2014 Mar 21]. Available from: http://www.proceedings.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=MSC0000000072005000100004&lng=en&nrm=abn
Available from: htt...
). However, in a study conducted with the application of the IRDl questionnaires to family videos, there were also no differences in the analysis of these items, which was attributed, in the study, to the possibility that the ability to respond to the calling by others is not yet affected in the autistic group from 0 to 3 months of age( 2727. Ward S, Gilmore L. The autistic behavioural indicators instrument (ABII): development and instrument utility in discriminating autistic disorder from speech and language impairment and typical development. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010;4:28-42. ).

From the findings presented, it appears that much of the content of the IRDIquestionnaire is consistently referenced in the literature, emphasizing signs of significant risk for ASD. Thus, it is suggested that this instrument may support future researches on the subject, particularly as to the early diagnosis of ASD.

This study has some limitations. The first of them refers to the sample number, which must be increased in future researches to verify if the results agree with those obtained here.

It is noteworthy that other researches with the instrument are in progress, including one comparing it to another instrument considered to be of goldstandard for diagnosis.

CONCLUSION

This research started from the finding that the delay in language development is one of the most common concerns of parents of children with ASD, which is why the Speech-Language Pathologist is, in many cases, the first professional sought by the family. Thus, the role of these professionals in identifying the potential risk for ASD and the importance of their conduct as to redirecting the patients toward relevant clinical care are evident.

Aiming to support and endorse such actions, this research presented a proposal for the adapting of the IRDI into a retrospective questionnaire for parents (IRDIquestionnaire).

It was found that the questionnaire items covered the main early clinical signs reported in the literature. It was also noticeable that the answers by the parents have pointed out significant differences between the SG and the CG.

The application of the IRDIquestionnaire also included an important recommendation as to identification, tracking, and sorting tools: ease of application and low cost, enabling their use on a large scale.

Other studies with the IRDIquestionnaire are recommended, aiming at the understanding of its scope and limitations. Therefore, it is suggested that studies with the IRDIquestionnaire should continue, so that it can be effectively validated and used as a tool for tracking ASD.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Volkmar FR, Pauls D. Autism. Lancet. 2003;362(9390):1133-41.
  • 2
    Thurm A, Lord C, Lee LC, Newschaffer C. Predictors of language acquisition in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1721-34.
  • 3
    Eigsti IM, Bennetto L, Dadlani MB. Beyond pragmatics: morphosyntactic development in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1007-23.
  • 4
    Weismer SE, Lord C, Esler A. Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:1259-73.
  • 5
    Matson JL, Wilkins J, Gonzalez M. Early identification and diagnosis in autism spectrum disorders in young children and infants: how early is too early? Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2008;2:75-84.
  • 6
    Wetherby AM, Woods J, Allen L, Cleary J, Dickinson H, Lord C. Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(5):473-93.
  • 7
    Dewrang P, Sandberg AD. Parental retrospective assessment of development and behavior in Asperger syndrome during the first 2 years of life. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010;4:461-73.
  • 8
    Horovitz M, Matson JL, Tuygin N, Beighley JS. The relationship between gender and age of first concern in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6:466-71.
  • 9
    Levy SE, Mandell DS, Schultz RT. Autism. Lancet. 2009; 374(9701):1627-38.
  • 10
    Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Lord C, Rogers S, Carter A, Carver L, et al. Clinical assessment and management of toddlers with suspected autism spectrum disorder: insights from studies of high-risk infants. Pediatrics. 2009;123:1383-91.
  • 11
    Matson JL, Beighley J, Turygin N. Autism diagnosis and screening: factors to consider in differential diagnosis. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6:19-24.
  • 12
    Itzchak EB, Zachor DA. Who benefits from early intervention in autism spectrum disorders? Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2011;5:345-50.
  • 13
    LoVullo SV, Matson JL. Development of a critical item algorithm for the baby and infant screen for children with autism traits. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6(1):378-84.
  • 14
    Reznick JS, Baranek GT, Watson LR, Crais ER. A parent-report instrument for identifying one-year-olds at risk for eventual diagnosis of autism: the first year inventory. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1691-710.
  • 15
    Rogers SJ, Hepburn S, Wehner E. Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2003;33(6):631-42.
  • 16
    Clifford SM, Dissanayake C. The early development of joint attention in infants with autistic disorder using home video observations and parental interview. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:791-805.
  • 17
    Sousa EC, Lima FT, Tamanaha AC, Perissinoto J, Azevedo MF, Chiari BM. A associação entre a suspeita inicial de perda auditiva e a ausência de comunicação verbal em crianças com transtornos do espectro autístico. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(4):487-90.
  • 18
    Kupfer MCM, Jerusalinsky AN, Bernardino LF, Wanderlet D, Rocha PSP, Molina SE, et al. Valor preditivo de indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil: um estudo a partir da teoria psicanalítica. Rev Latinoam Psicopatol Fundam. 2010;13(1):31-52.
  • 19
    American Psychiatric Association. Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais - DSM-IV-TR - Texto Revisado. Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas; 2002.
  • 20
    Sato FP. Validação da versão em português de um questionário para avaliação de autismo infantil [dissertação]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2008.
  • 21
    Pesaro ME. Alcance e limites teórico-metodológicos da pesquisa multicêntrica de indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2010.
  • 22
    Lerner R. Indicadores clínicos de risco para o desenvolvimento infantil - IRDI: verificação da capacidade discriminativa entre autismo, retardo mental e normalidade [livre-docência]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2011.
  • 23
    Pereira A, Riesgo RS, Wagner MB. Autismo infantil: tradução e validação da Childhood Autism Rating Scale para uso no Brasil. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2008;84(6):487-94.
  • 24
    Shopler E, Reichler RJ, Devellis RF, Daly K. Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). J Autism Dev Disord. 1980;10:91-103.
  • 25
    Poon KK, Watson LR, Baranek GT, Poe MD. To what extent do joint attention, imitation, and object play behaviors in infancy predict later communication and intellectual functioning in ASD? J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(6):1064-74.
  • 26
    Brisson J, Serres J, Gattegno MP, Adrien JL. Étude des troubles précoces du contact social à partir de l'ánalyse des films familiaux chez des nourrissons de la naissance à 6 mois ultérieurement diagnostiqués autistes. Devenir. 2011;23(1):87-106.
  • 27
    Ward S, Gilmore L. The autistic behavioural indicators instrument (ABII): development and instrument utility in discriminating autistic disorder from speech and language impairment and typical development. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010;4:28-42.
  • 28
    Maestro S, Muratori F, Cesari A, Cavallaro M, Paziente A, Pecini E, et al. Course of autism signs in the first year of life. Psychopathology. 2005;38:26-31.
  • 29
    Toth K, Munson J, Meltzoff AN, Dawson G. Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism spectrum disorder: joint attention, imitation, and toy play. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:993-1005.
  • 30
    Laznik MC, Maestro S, Muratori F, Parlato E. Interações sonoras entre bebês que se tornaram autistas e seus pais. In: Anais do Colóquio franco-brasileiro sobre a clínica com bebês. Paris; 2005 [cited 2014 Mar 21]. Available from: http://www.proceedings.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=MSC0000000072005000100004&lng=en&nrm=abn
    » Available from: http://www.proceedings.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=MSC0000000072005000100004&lng=en&nrm=abn
  • Study carried out at the Pontifícia Universidade Católica of São Paulo - PUC-SP - São Paulo (SP), Brazil.
  • *
    FPM and MCC were responsible for defining the design of the study; FPM performed the data collection and literature survey, which were analyzed and discussed together with MCC and RRRP.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Apr 2014

History

  • Received
    11 July 2013
  • Accepted
    13 Mar 2014
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br