Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Effects of specific hypnotic suggestions on mechanical and thermal sensitivity of healthy volunteers: randomized and double-blind study

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES:

Hypnotic suggestions for hypoalgesia or analgesia are efficient for relieving different pain conditions, presenting few or no side effects. However, little is known about its direct effect on the modulation of peripheral nociception. The goal of this study was to evaluate the mechanical and thermal response after specific hypnotic suggestions in healthy volunteers.

METHODS:

This is a randomized double-blinded controlled trial that aimed to evaluate both mechanical and thermal nociception after specific hypnotic suggestions in healthy volunteers. For this, twenty-seven participants were enrolled, according to the following eligibility criteria: age between 18-65 years and absence of pain complaints or psychological disorders. After signed Free Informed Consent Term (FICT) the participants were divided by a computer-generated randomization in three groups: sham group (no induction of hypnosis), hypnosis-induced pain group and hypnosis-induced analgesia group. Susceptibility to hypnosis was assessed through the Waterloo-Stanford Group C (WSGC) scale of hypnotic susceptibility and outcomes included evaluation of questionnaires (Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale and Short Form Brief Pain Inventory) as well as the examination of mechanical and thermal nociception through the Quantitative Sensory Testing (QST), a tool widely used to investigate somatosensory sensitivity by assessing functions of small A-δ and C nerve sensory fibers, before and after specific hypnotic suggestion for pain and analgesia made by a qualified hypnotherapist.

RESULTS:

Data demonstrated that specific hypnotic suggestions induced significant changes in mechanical and thermal sensitivity. The pain group revealed an increase in mechanical hyperalgesia and allodynia, while the analgesia group increased pain thresholds to thermal stimulations, being conditioned to withstand temperature changes after hypnosis, demonstrating a modulatory effect for both pain and analgesia sensations in healthy volunteers.

CONCLUSION:

The evidence presented in this study supports the use of the hypnosis technique as an auxiliary tool in clinical practice.

HIGHLIGHTS

  • Specific hypnotic suggestions can modulate peripheral nociception in healthy subjects.

  • Data show a modulatory effect for both pain and analgesia sensations.

  • Hypnosis can be considered a feasible technique for the clinical pain management.

INTRODUCTION

Hypnosis is considered a psychological intervention indicated to treat diseases such as depression, obesity, and phobias, and is one of the oldest forms for pain management11 Facco E, Casiglia E, Zanette G, Testoni I. On the way of liberation from suffering and pain: role of hypnosis in palliative care. Ann Palliat Med. 2018;7(1):63-74.. Non-pharmacological nature, convenience of delivering treatment, and few side effects are major advantages of this therapy22 Jensen M P, Patterson DR. Hypnotic approaches for chronic pain management: clinical implications of recent research findings. Am Psychol. 2014;69(2):167-77.. It can be delivered in a pre-recorded format in the presence of a hypnotherapist. Despite much evidence of its efficacy in chronic and acute pain therapy, little is known about its direct effect on the modulation of nociception33 Morone NE, Greco CM. Mind-body interventions for chronic pain in older adults: a structured review. Pain Med. 2007;8(4):359-75., 44 Bernardy K, Füber N, Klose P, Häuser W. Efficacy of hypnosis/guided imagery in fibromyalgia syndrome--a systematic review and meta-analysis of controlled trials. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2011;15;12:133., 55 Wetering EJ, Lemmens KM, Nieboer A P, Huijsman R. Cognitive and behavioral interventions for the management of chronic neuropathic pain in adults--a systematic review. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(7):670-81., 66 Tompson T, Terhune DB, Oram C, Sharangparni J, Rouf R, Solmi M, Veronese N, Stubbs B. The effectiveness of hypnosis for pain relief: A systematic review and meta-a-nalysis of 85 controlled experimental trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;99:298-310., 77 Milling LS, Valentine KE, LoStimolo LM, Nett AM, McCarley HS. Hypnosis and the Alleviation of Clinical Pain: A Comprehensive Meta-Analysis. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 2021;69(3):297-322..

There are different approaches to delivering hypnosis therapy. Many applications involve minimal induction with suggestions for relaxation and the use of imagery, that is often referred to as “neutral hypnosis”22 Jensen M P, Patterson DR. Hypnotic approaches for chronic pain management: clinical implications of recent research findings. Am Psychol. 2014;69(2):167-77.,88 Cardeña E. The phenomenology of deep hypnosis: quiescent and physically active. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 2005;53(1):37-59.,99 Cardeña E, Jönsson P, Terhune DB, Marcusson-Clavertz D. The neurophenomenology of neutral hypnosis. Cortex. 2013;49(2):375-85.. In other cases, a specifically focused analgesia suggestion is employed, aiming to alter perceptual experience and behavior22 Jensen M P, Patterson DR. Hypnotic approaches for chronic pain management: clinical implications of recent research findings. Am Psychol. 2014;69(2):167-77.. Some studies have shown that hypnotic relaxation is equally effective as an analgesic suggestion, while others have indicated that specific analgesic cues are more effective66 Tompson T, Terhune DB, Oram C, Sharangparni J, Rouf R, Solmi M, Veronese N, Stubbs B. The effectiveness of hypnosis for pain relief: A systematic review and meta-a-nalysis of 85 controlled experimental trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;99:298-310.,1010 Kiernan BD, Dane JR, Phillips LH, Price DD. Hypnotic analgesia reduces R-III nociceptive reflex: further evidence concerning the multifactorial nature of hypnotic analgesia. Pain. 1995;60(1):39-47., 1111 Castel A, Pérez M, Sala J, Padrol A, Rull M. Effect of hypnotic suggestion on fbromyalgic pain: comparison between hypnosis and relaxation. Eur J Pain. 2007;11(4):463-8., 1212 Zachariae R, Andersen OK, Bjerring P, Jørgensen MM, Arendt-Nielsen L. Effects of an opioid antagonist on pain intensity and withdrawal reflexes during induction of hypnotic analgesia in high- and low-hypnotizable volunteers. Eur J Pain. 1998;2(1):25-34.. Nonetheless, the analgesic effect of hypnosis can be so powerful that surgical procedures have even been performed without anesthesia1313 Facco E, Pasquali S, Zanette G, Casiglia E. (2013). Hypnosis as sole anaesthesia for skin tumour removal in a patient with multiple chemical sensitivity. Anaesthesia. 2013;68(9):961-5.. In fact, several studies support hypnosis as a potential approach to relieve unaddressed pain and anxiety in burn patients undergoing wound care, managing labor pain, and fibromyalgia44 Bernardy K, Füber N, Klose P, Häuser W. Efficacy of hypnosis/guided imagery in fibromyalgia syndrome--a systematic review and meta-analysis of controlled trials. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2011;15;12:133.,1414 Provençal SC, Bond S, Rizkallah E, El-Baalbaki G. Hypnosis for burn wound care pain and anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Burns. 2018;44(8):1870-81.,1515 Madden K, Middleton P, Cyna AM, Matthewson M, Jones L. Hypnosis for pain management during labour and childbirth. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016;(5):CD009356..

Not all human beings respond equally to hypnotic intervention. Hypnotic susceptibility determines direct analgesic efficacy that is critical to the treatment success66 Tompson T, Terhune DB, Oram C, Sharangparni J, Rouf R, Solmi M, Veronese N, Stubbs B. The effectiveness of hypnosis for pain relief: A systematic review and meta-a-nalysis of 85 controlled experimental trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;99:298-310.. Susceptibility scoring is described as the aggregation of behavioral responses to a series of individual suggestions66 Tompson T, Terhune DB, Oram C, Sharangparni J, Rouf R, Solmi M, Veronese N, Stubbs B. The effectiveness of hypnosis for pain relief: A systematic review and meta-a-nalysis of 85 controlled experimental trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;99:298-310.. Several scales have been generated such as German Norms for Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility (HGSHS)1616 Bongartz W. German norms for the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1985;33(2):131-9., Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (SHSS), and a more recent Waterloo-Stanford Group C (WSGC)1717 Bowers KS. Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C: manual and response booklet. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1998;46(3):250-68.. WSGC consists of hypnotic induction followed by the presentation of 12 hypnotic suggestions (hand lowering, arm rigidity, and immobilization, for example), which classifies individuals as low, medium, or high hypnotic susceptibility, describing not only their ability to enter a hypnotic state but also a correlation with their postural control1818 Carli G, Manzoni D, Santarcangelo EL. Hypnotizability-related integration of perception and action. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 2008;25(7-8):1065-76.. Indeed, pain reduction was found to be more effective in individuals who were considered more susceptible (highly hypnotizable) to hypnosis, when hypnotic intervention was performed for analgesia1919 Benhaiem JM, Attal N, Chauvin M, Brasseur L, Bouhassira D. Local and remote effects of hypnotic suggestions of analgesia. Pain. 2001;89(2-3):167-73.,2020 Paoletti G, Varanini M, Balocchi R, Morizzo C, Palombo C, Santarcangelo EL. Cardiovascular and respiratory correlates of deep nociceptive stimulation, suggestions for analgesia, pain imagery and cognitive load as a function of hypnotizability. Brain Res Bull. 2010;82(1-2):65-73..

To better understand the hypnotic analgesia, it is important to differentiate cognitive and behavioral elements from peripheral sensory perception2121 Gay MC, Philippot P, Luminet O. Differential effectiveness of psychological interventions for reducing osteoarthritis pain: a comparison of Erikson [correction of Erickson] hypnosis and Jacobson relaxation. Eur J Pain. 2002;6(1):1-16.. Hence, this study examined the effects of specific hypnotic suggestionsin peripheral sensitivity in a healthy volunteer, with no current pain complaints.

The goal of this study was to evaluate the mechanical and thermal response after specific hypnotic suggestions in healthy volunteers.

METHODS

This is a randomized, double-blinded controlled clinical trial. The Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT), CONSORT for Abstracts (CONSORT-A) and the Template for Intervention Description and Replication (TIDieR) checklist and guide were followed for the elaboration of this manuscript.

Twenty-seven volunteers were recruited from the population of healthy university students by advertisements posted in the university. The following inclusion criteria were applied: age between 18 and 65 years, and absence of complaints of pain or psychological disorders.

Subjects with a history of major psychiatric disease, substance abuse, or those unable to understand the consent form, individuals complaining of pain, and severe systemic, metabolic, or neurological disease capable of influencing the Quantitative Sensory Testing (QST) were excluded from this study. The protocol and informed consent forms were reviewed and approved by the local Ethics Committee on Human Research (CEPSH-ICB; CAAE: 87585918.2.0000.5467).

After all participants signed the Free and Informed Consent Term (FICT), a computer-generated randomization (Excel software) was used to assign participant to three different groups, this information was kept confidential in an opaque envelope. The groups were namely a) sham group (no induction of hypnosis), b) hypnosis-induced pain group, and c) hypnosis-induced analgesia group, for these two last groups hypnotic suggestion for pain and analgesia was given targeting the increase and decrease of the subject’s pain and control over their sensations.

Following the initial procedures, the hypnotherapist was blinded to participant hypnosis susceptibility score, and the outcome assessor was blinded to group allocation. To objectively assess peripheral sensitization, questionnaires and Quantitative Sensory Testing (QST) were used. This analysis quantifies the peripheral pain sensitivity transmitted by thin (C) or thick sensory fbers (small A-δ) of the peripheral nervous system (PNS). This allows determination of basal and pain thresholds to mechanical and thermal stimuli, in addition to enabling the detection of certain conditions, like hyperalgesia or hyperpathia2222 Arendt-Nielsen L, Yarnitsky D. Experimental and clinical applications of quantitative sensory testing applied to skin, muscles and viscera. J Pain. 2009;10(6):556-72.,2323 Siviero M, Teixeira MJ, de Siqueira JT, Siqueira SR. Somesthetic, gustatory, olfactory function and salivary fow in patients with neuropathic trigeminal pain. Oral Dis. 2010;16(5):482-7.. All eligible volunteers received an explanation of the study objectives. All steps conducted in this study were represented in the figure 1 which contains a detailed explanation of the validated questionnaires applied to assess comorbidities and pain symptoms.

Figure 1
Flowchart of the study population and experiment design

Questionnaires

In addition to standard demographic questions, the following questionnaires were used to assess comorbidities and pain symptoms: Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT)2424 Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor T F, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption--II. Addiction. 1993;88(6):791-804., Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)2525 Castro MM, Quarantini L, Batista-Neves S, Kraychete DC, Daltro C, Miranda-Scippa A. Validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale in patients with chronic pain. Rev Bras Anestesiol. 2006;56(5):470-7. for anxiety and depression disorders, and Brief Pain Inventory – Short Form (BPI)2626 Ferreira KA, Teixeira MJ, Mendonza TR, Cleeland CS. Validation of brief pain inventory to Brazilian patients with pain. Support Care Cancer. 2011;19(4):505-11. for somatosensory symptoms of pain. Details of these validated questionnaires used routinely are described in figure 1.

Hypnotic susceptibility

Waterloo-Stanford Group C Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility (WSGC)1717 Bowers KS. Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C: manual and response booklet. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1998;46(3):250-68. was used to determine the hypnotic susceptibility on a score from 0 to 12. Testing for hypnotic susceptibility was performed by a qualified hypnotherapist. The procedure was verbally standardized for all volunteers through a headphone containing four sensory families: psycho-imaginary, psycho-conflictive, polyvalent, and sensory. A sophisticated infrastructure was not required, only a quiet room with a comfortable chair.

After a hypnotic induction of about 20 minutes followed by the presentation of 12 hypnotic suggestions (1: Hand lowering; 2: Moving hands together; 3: Mosquito hallucination; 4: Taste hallucination; 5: arm rigidity; 6: dream about hypnosis; 7: arm immobilization; 8: age regression; 9: music hallucination - hear jingle bells; 10: negative visual hallucination; 11: posthypnotic suggestion - draw a doodle of a tree on the response booklet, and 12: posthypnotic amnesia), all volunteers undergone the hypnotic susceptibility scale to be classified as low (0-3 points), moderate (4-8 points), or high (9-12 points) susceptibility to hypnosis according to their score.

Specific hypnotic suggestions

Hypnosis was performed by a qualified hypnotherapist with 27 years working at the Hospital das Clínicas (Clinical Hospital) of the Universidade de São Paulo (USP - University of São Paulo), in face-to-face format. No participant had previous experience with hypnosis. A blinded assistant recorded all data. Hypnosis protocol consisted of two phases: induction and hypnotic suggestions. Hypnotic induction was standardized to be equally applied to all subjects, except to the sham group. The standard hypnotic protocol begins with an induction that was associated with breathing and relaxation, where subjects received suggestions to focus their attention on a single stimulus until reaching a trance state. This phase lasts around 10 minutes.

After the induction, the hypnotic suggestion phase started, working on the imaginary, giving color, size, and shape to the sensation referred according to the hypnotic suggestion for pain and analgesia was given targeting the increase and decrease of the subject’s pain and controls over their sensations in those groups, rescuing and using an interpretation already known to the hypnotized person2727 Patterson DR, Jensen M P. Hypnosis and clinical pain. Psychol Bull. 2003;129(4):495-521.. For example, as the area evaluated was the hand, in the pain group, the participant was asked to imagine the hand on fire, and the color and shape of the fire were asked, thus inducing an increase in sensitivity in that region.

In the analgesia group, the participant was asked to imagine or remember the sensation of sitting on the hand for a long period, causing tingling and numbness, inducing a loss of sensitivity in the area. The control group was undergone to the same procedures as other groups, but without induction or programming phase applied. They were asked to control breathing and relax. The experimental manipulations (induction + programming/suggestions) lasted for 20 min.

Quantitative Sensory Test (QST)

All QST were performed according to the study2828 Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tölle TR, Treede -DR, Beyer A, Binder A, Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Bötefür IC, Braune S, Flor H, Huge V, Klug R, Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Maihöfner C, Rolko C, Schaub C, Scherens A, Sprenger T, Valet M, Wasserka B. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006;123(3):231-43. at baseline and after hypnosis intervention on glabrous skin of the participant’s left hand thenar eminence. During the test, participants were seated in a comfortable chair in a quiet room and kept their eyes closed during evaluations. All participants were subjected to a basal QST session (before hypnosis induction) and post-hypnotic intervention.

a) Mechanical detection and mechanical pain threshold: was measured using a standardized set of modified von Frey hairs that exert forces between 0.25 and 1079 mN (Somedic AB™, Horby, Sweden). The contact area of von Frey hairs with the skin (hand) had uniform size and shape (rounded tip, 0.5 mm in diameter) to avoid sharp edges that would facilitate nociceptor activation. The assessment started with the lowest filament. One affirmative answer meant perception of the filament. If the filament used was not felt, a filament with higher graduation would be used. The result was analyzed from the first filament perceived disregarding the filaments that were not perceived. Mechanical detection thresholds (MDT) and mechanical pain thresholds (MPT) were defined as the lowest pressure that generated a sensation of touch or pain, respectively. Mechanical pain sensitivity (MPS) was tested using the von Frey filament four times higher than that used for MPT, in addition, the pain intensity was also rated by a visual analog scale (VAS).

b) Dynamic allodynia: stimulus-response-functions for dynamic mechanical allodynia (DMA) were determined using a standardized brush (Somedic™, Sweden) exerting a force of 200–400 mN, applied only once. The subject was asked to provide a pain rating for each stimulus on a 0-100 mm numerical rating scale (NRS), with 0 indicating ‘‘no pain’’, and 100 indicating ‘‘most intense pain imaginable’’.

c) Wind-up ratio: the wind-up ratio is defined as the perceptual correlation of temporal pain summation for repetitive mechanical stimuli. In this test of temporal summation, the perceived magnitude of a single von Frey stimulus was compared to that of a train of 10 stimuli of the same force repeated at a 1/s rate (166 mN). The train of von Frey stimuli was given within a small area of 1 cm22 Jensen M P, Patterson DR. Hypnotic approaches for chronic pain management: clinical implications of recent research findings. Am Psychol. 2014;69(2):167-77. and the subject was asked to provide a rating representing the pain by the end of the training using VAS. The mean pain rating of repeated over single stimuli was calculated as the wind-up ratio (WUR).

d) Thermal detection and thermal pain thresholds: tests for thermal sensation were performed based on a TSA 2001-II (MEDOC, Israel) thermal sensory testing device. Cold detection threshold (CDT) and warm detection threshold (WDT) were measured first followed by heat pain threshold (HPT) and cold pain threshold (CPT), respectively, using the Method of Limits2929 Mücke M, Cuhls H, Radbruch L, Baron R, Maier C, Tölle T, Treede RD, Rolke R. Quantitative sensory testing (QST). English version. Schmerz. 2021;35(Suppl 3)153-60.. For the measurement of CDT and WDT, subjects received four successive ramps of gradually decreasing or increasing temperature, starting from a resting neutral temperature of 32°C, at a rate of 1°C per second2121 Gay MC, Philippot P, Luminet O. Differential effectiveness of psychological interventions for reducing osteoarthritis pain: a comparison of Erikson [correction of Erickson] hypnosis and Jacobson relaxation. Eur J Pain. 2002;6(1):1-16.,3030 de Melo DM, Barbosa AJ. (2015). O uso do Mini-Exame do Estado Mental em pesquisas com idosos no Brasil: uma revisão sistemática. Cien Saude Colet. 2015;20(12):3865-76.. Subjects were instructed to press a response button when a thermal sensation (either cold or warm) was first perceived. Pressing the button resulted in the automatic recording of the threshold temperature and returning of the thermode to the neutral temperature. Thermal ramps were repeated every 4-6s. A similar procedure was applied for determining CPT and HPT, but the stimuli were applied at 20 to 30 sec intervals, and the subjects were instructed to press the response button immediately after perceiving the thermal sensation as painful. Thermal thresholds were determined by averaging the reading of the four successive stimuli, discarding the reading most separated from the mean, and recalculating the average of the three remaining temperatures3131 Velasco R, Videla S, Villoria J, Ortiz E, Navarro X, Bruna J. Reliability and accuracy of quantitative sensory testing for oxaliplatin-induced neurotoxicity. Acta Neurol Scand. 2015;131(5):282-9.. The intensity of pain evoked by suprathreshold stimuli was also assessed. For warm (WST) and cold (CST) suprathreshold, there was a constant increase (46°C and 48°C) or decrease (10°C and 5°C) from neutral temperature with a speed of 2°C/s, respectively. The means of the two VAS scores obtained during the suprathreshold stimuli were assessed.

Statistical analysis

Continuous quantitative variables were expressed as means, as standard error of the mean (SEM), or as standard deviation (SD), while nominal categorical variables were expressed in percentage (%). Data was assessed for normal distribution, failing which non-parametric tests were applied. Wilcoxon test assessed pre- and post-hypnosis of each group and the Kruskal-Wallis test was applied for statistical comparisons of the three groups. Data were analyzed on SPSS Statistics 20™ (IBM, Seattle, USA) and graphs were generated in GraphPad Prism™ (Version 8, GraphPad Software Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). A p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

RESULTS

Epidemiological data of healthy individuals

Of the twenty-seven healthy participants, 10 men (37%; mean age: 35.50±5.195) and 17 women (63%; mean age: 36.82±3.536) were enrolled in this study (Figure 2A). According to the sociodemographic information, 74% (n= 20) of the participants self-declared as white, 18% (n= 5) as black, and the remaining 8% (n= 2) as Asian or aboriginal. Most of the participants (63%, n=19) were classified as class C (21.56±1.029) with an income corresponding to 4-10 minimum wages. Regarding alcohol abuse, the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) demonstrated that 100% (n= 27) of the participants were at low alcoholism risk or abstaining (Figure 2A).

Figure 2
Epidemiological data and hypnotic susceptibility of healthy individuals

The presence of pain over the previous 24 hours was assessed by using Brief Pain Inventory (BPI), demonstrating the sensory dimension (average and current pain), and the impact of pain on quality of life (general activities) through the Visual Analog Scale (VAS). All individuals had a null or very low (less than 5) pain score (Figure 2A), making them eligible for the study. Still, when evaluated the hypnotic susceptibility scale most of the participants, 44% (n = 12), were classified as moderately susceptible (6.16±0.34) to hypnosis. The remaining part of the sample demonstrated high (9.71±0.28, 26%, n= 7)) or low (2.12±0.39, 30%, n= 8) susceptibility to hypnosis (Figure 2B). There were no differences among genders when evaluated.

General characteristics of volunteers were demonstrated as absolute numbers (n), percentages (%), mean and standard error of the mean (SEM) according to evaluation through validated questionnaires. (A) indicates the epidemiological data and (B) the hypnotic susceptibility of healthy subjects. All volunteers were submitted to the hypnotic susceptibility scale performed by a qualified hypnotherapist; the classification consists of low hypnotizability (0-3 points), moderate hypnotizability (4-8 points), or high hypnotizability (9-12 points). Neither the participants, the hypnotherapist nor the QST examiners were informed about the results of the susceptibility evaluation. GraphPad Prism 8.0.

Quality of life of individuals subjected to hypnosis procedure

The assessment of emotional aspects and related effects on the participant’s quality of life using the HADS showed that 52% (n= 14) had symptoms of anxiety (n= 6), depression (n= 3) or both (n= 5) concomitantly (Table 1). Of these participants with emotional disturbances, most of them (50%, n= 7) were moderately susceptible to hypnosis.

Table 1
Assessment of the presence of anxiety and depression symptoms in healthy individuals undergoing hypnosis protocol

Data correspond to the mean±sem of 27 patients expressed as a percentage (%) extracted from the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) questionnaire.

Effect of specific hypnotic suggestion on peripheral sensitivity

Subjects were arranged in three different groups: sham (n=9), pain (n=11), and analgesia (n=7). Results of mechanical and thermal quantitative sensory testing are shown in tables 2 and 3, respectively.

Table 2
Mechanical QST-parameters of healthy subjects
Table 3
Thermal QST-parameters of healthy subjects

The subjects were divided into sham (n= 9), pain (n=11), and analgesia (n=7) groups, and mechanical exteroceptive sensitivity were obtained through von Frey filaments measured in millinewtons (mN). The detection values for temperature were measured on hand, in celsius degrees (ºC). Visual Analogue Scale (VAS): 0 to 100 mm, was used for mechanical and thermal mensuration. Means and standard deviation (SD) of raw data were compared using a non-parametric Wilcoxon test for comparison before (Pre-hypnosis) and after (Post-hypnosis) hypnosis; p-value considered significant was <0.05.

Pain caused by mechanical hyperalgesia through repetitive painful stimulation was exacerbated by hypnosis in the pain group, once variation in pain intensity was positive after specific hypnotic suggestion (MPS, pre: 8.00±10.667/post: 15.27±12.900, p=0.011 – Table 2). In contrast, no significant result was observed in the analgesia group (MPS, pre: 9.42±12.67/post: 0.57±1.133, p=0.058 - Table 2). The analysis between groups showed no difference between analgesia and pain groups (p=0.0785 – Kruskal-Wallis test – Table 4).

Table 4
Interaction of mechanical and thermal quantitative sensory testing parameters of healthy subjects between groups

Additionally, the pain group reported greater pain intensity in response to the brush touch (DMA, pre: 0.00±0.000/ post: 10.18±11.694, p=0.012 – Table 2). This was also observed comparing with the analgesia group (p=0.0029 – Kruskal-Wallis test, Table 4). In turn, the sham and analgesia groups reported an almost zero pain intensity in response to this stimulus, as expected considering the evaluation of healthy individuals. Regarding the wind-up ratio, statistical differences (p=0.028) were observed in the analgesia group (WUR, pre: 2.652±1.650/ post: 0.00±0.000-Table 2). After being subjected to hypnosis, the analgesia group presented statistical differences (p=0.0305) in relation to the pain group (Kruskal-Wallis test, Table 4).

Furthermore, differences in mechanical detection (MDT) and mechanical pain (MPT) thresholds were not observed intra-groups for any of them. However, when assessing the interaction between groups, a significant difference in the pain versus the analgesia group occurred in the MPT (p=0.0294– Kruskal-Wallis test, Table 4). Finally, regarding the mechanical exteroceptive sensitivity assessment, as expected, the sham group (no induction of hypnosis) showed no statistical difference after hypnosis for any of the assessed parameters, as shown in Table 2. The measurement of exteroceptive thermal sensitivity demonstrated that the individuals presented normal range detection thresholds with no alteration after hypnosis in almost all groups, except for the pain group, which presented a significant difference for the warm detection threshold (WDT, pre: 34.02±0.895/post: 33.59±0.592, p=0.041 – Table 3) after specific hypnotic suggestion. In addition, a statistical difference was observed in relation to the analgesia group (p=0.0035–Kruskal-Wallis test, Table 4).

The subjects were divided in sham (n= 9), pain (n=11) and analgesia (n=7) groups. Kruskal-Wallis test was performed for statistical comparisons of the three groups pre and post-hypnosis. A p-value <0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Moreover, significant differences in cold and heat pain thresholds was found for both the pain and analgesia groups. The pain group presented a decrease in heat pain threshold (HPT, pre: 44.59±3.898/post: 39.21±4.428, p=0.004 – Table 3) after hypnosis, which means that they began to feel pain earlier, responding to the painful stimulus at lower temperatures than in the pre-hypnosis. The same behavior occurs for the cold stimulus, the pain group also presented a decrease in cold pain threshold (CPT, pre: 12.58±10.583/post: 23.08±7.243, p=0.008 – Table 3) after hypnosis. Thereby, for assessing cold pain, the thermode temperature (t) varies between 32 to 0 Celsius degrees, meaning that once again, the subjects began to feel pain earlier, responding to the painful stimulus at lower temperatures than in the pre-hypnosis. In contrast, no significant changes were observed in the exteroceptive sensation of the sham group, even for warm or cold sensations.

The analgesia group showed an opposite frame with the subjects who were hypnotized to not feel pain presenting an increase of heat (HPT, pre: 41.51±3.456/post: 45.48±3.251, p=0.018 – Table 3) and cold pain thresholds (CPT, pre: 18.28±8.290/ post: 14.31±10.310, p=0.034 – Table 3) after specific hypnotic suggestion. This means that they respond to pain stimulus later, being conditioned to withstand temperature changes.

Pain intensity was also assessed based on the VAS. For thermal stimuli, a variation in temperature occurred at 46-48˚C for warm sensation and at 10-5˚C for cold sensation. These temperatures can induce thermal hyperalgesia in both cases. Variations in pain intensity were positive for the pain group after hypnosis (WST, pre: 38.65±30.862/post: 54.00±27.575, p=0.009; CST, pre: 38.90±30.623/post: 55.15±27.633, p=0.008 – Table 3) for both warm and cold suprathresholds. However, it was negative for the analgesia group (WST, pre: 45.64±26.087/ post: 31.07±26.168, p=0.018; CST, pre: 41.50±26.229/post: 29.35±26.305, p=0.018 – Table 3) at all assessed temperatures. Such results indicate that the volunteers hypnotized to feel pain indeed reported hyperalgesia after specific hypnotic suggestion, meanwhile the pain intensity was lower for the analgesia group. No difference was observed for the sham group, neither warm nor cold suprathresholds.

DISCUSSION

In this study, hypnosis was applied to healthy participants, who presented low risk for alcohol abuse or were abstinent, respecting the inclusion criteria. The sample was focused on healthy individuals that presented null or very low pain scores, making them eligible for the study. Data presented herein revealed that specific hypnotic suggestions altered exteroceptive sensitivity, either mechanical or thermal, for both pain and analgesia in healthy subjects. Regarding mechanical nociception, the results indicate that hypnosis was able to induce hyperalgesia and allodynia in the pain group, resulting in intensification of pain. On the other hand, the analgesia group became less sensitive to repeated induction of pain stimuli after hypnosis. Hypnosis can be considered as a procedure in which an intentional introspective mental activity is induced and guided by a hypnotherapist that engenders relevant changes of experience and bodily functions3232 Nash MR, Barnier AJ. (Eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research and Practice. New York: Oxford University Press. Reviewed by Eric K. Willmarth, PhD, Saybrook Graduate School and Research Center, San Francisco, CA. 2008.. Hypnosis is known as a therapy that can intentionally change sensory inputs, feelings, mental representations, and behavioral and neurovegetative responses3333 Conversa G, Facco E, Leoni MLG, Buonocore M, Bagnasco R, Angelini L, Demartini L, Spiegel D. Quantitative Sensory Testing (QST) Estimation of regional cutaneous thermal sensitivity during waking state, neutral hypnosis, and temperature specific suggestions. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 2019;67(3):364-81..

Regarding sensory inputs, hypnosis is considered a powerful analgesic tool, able to increase the pain threshold up to the level of surgical anesthesia11 Facco E, Casiglia E, Zanette G, Testoni I. On the way of liberation from suffering and pain: role of hypnosis in palliative care. Ann Palliat Med. 2018;7(1):63-74.,1313 Facco E, Pasquali S, Zanette G, Casiglia E. (2013). Hypnosis as sole anaesthesia for skin tumour removal in a patient with multiple chemical sensitivity. Anaesthesia. 2013;68(9):961-5. as well as modulate proprioceptive and sensory, nonpainful, inputs3434 Caratelli E, Menzocchi M, Carli G, Fontani G, Santarcangelo E. Is high hypnotizability a trouble in balance control? Eur J Appl Physiol. 2010;108(1):201-6.,3535 Menzocchi M, Paoletti G, Carli G, Scattina E, Manzoni D, Santarcangelo EL. Hypnotizability -related effects of vestibular impairment on posture and locomotion. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 2010;58(3):329-44.. The results of this study demonstrate that specific hypnotic suggestions increased pain thresholds in the analgesia group after both heat and cold stimulations, meaning that these subjects became more resistant to pain sensations after hypnosis. In addition, it decreased pain thresholds in the pain group after both heat and cold stimulation, causing those individuals to be less resistant to pain. These data agree with those from randomized controlled studies demonstrating that hypnotic suggestion could improve pain conditions and analgesia2727 Patterson DR, Jensen M P. Hypnosis and clinical pain. Psychol Bull. 2003;129(4):495-521.,3636 Montgomery GH, David D, Winkel G, Silverstein JH, Bovbjerg DH. The effectiveness of adjunctive hypnosis with surgical patients: a meta-analysis. Anesth Analg. 2002;94(6):1639-45..

The results of this study also indicated that hypnotic suggestion might be an effective procedure for alleviating pain perception in experimental models3737 Vanhaudenhuyse A, Boly M, Balteau E, Schnakers C, Moonen G, Luxen A, Lamy M, Degueldre C, Brichant J F, Maquet P, Laureys S, Faymonville ME. Pain and non-pain processing during hypnosis: a thulium-YAG event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2009;47(3):1047-54.,3838 Brunoni AR, Mofa AH, Fregni F, Palm U, Padberg F, Blumberger DM, Daskalakis ZJ, Bennabi D, Haffen E, Alonzo A, Loo CK. Transcranial direct current stimulation for acute major depressive episodes: meta-analysis of individual patient data. Br J Psychiatry. 2016;208(6):522-31.. However, no changes were observed for the detection parameters of basal thresholds neither for the pain nor the analgesia group. These results can be related to the hypnotic suggestion being made on participants to induce either pain or analgesia, thus interfering with sensory, while detection thresholds should remain unchanged once hypnosis was supposed to change only pain perception and not basal sensitivity. These data are supported by clinical studies demonstrating that hypnotic relaxation without a specific analgesic suggestion result in thermal and mechanical detection, but not pain threshold changes, thus demonstrating that a relaxation suggestion has no genuine effect on sensory pain thresholds3939 Kramer S, Zims R, Simang M, Rüger L, Irnich D. Hypnotic relaxation results in elevated thresholds of sensory detection but not of pain detection. BMC Complement Altern Med. Dec 2014;15;14:496.. Herein, this study administered hypnotic suggestions, once subjects were specifically hypnotized to feel pain, revealing that specific suggestions are essential to hypnosis’s effect on pain sensitivity. The exact mechanisms by which hypnosis can change pain perception are still being subjected to research, however, significant changes were revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in the insula, prefrontal, parietal, and anterior cingulate (ACC) cortices, areas involved in painful modulation4040 Derbyshire S W, Whalley MG, Stenger VA, Oakley DA. Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain. Neuroimage. 2004;23(1):392-401.,4141 Jiang H, White M P, Greicius MD, Waelde LC, Spiegel D. Brain activity and functional connectivity associated with hypnosis. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27(8):4083-93., thus demonstrating that these regions influence pain thresholds.

It is worth highlighting that the quantitative sensory test (QST) is a psychophysical method that allows quantifying positive and negative phenomena of exteroceptive sensitivity transmitted by the thin or thick fibers of the peripheral nervous system (PNS)2222 Arendt-Nielsen L, Yarnitsky D. Experimental and clinical applications of quantitative sensory testing applied to skin, muscles and viscera. J Pain. 2009;10(6):556-72.,2323 Siviero M, Teixeira MJ, de Siqueira JT, Siqueira SR. Somesthetic, gustatory, olfactory function and salivary fow in patients with neuropathic trigeminal pain. Oral Dis. 2010;16(5):482-7.. This instrument has been widely used in studies that are focused on explaining and understanding how pain mechanisms work, such as in neuropathic pain or orofacial pain, for example2828 Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tölle TR, Treede -DR, Beyer A, Binder A, Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Bötefür IC, Braune S, Flor H, Huge V, Klug R, Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Maihöfner C, Rolko C, Schaub C, Scherens A, Sprenger T, Valet M, Wasserka B. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006;123(3):231-43.,4242 Baad-Hansen L, Abrahamsen R, Zachariae R, List T, Svensson P. Somatosensory sensitivity in patients with persistent idiopathic orofacial pain is associated with pain relief from hypnosis and relaxation. Clin J Pain. 2013;29(6):518-26., enabling to examine both cutaneous and deep pain sensitivity, including cerebral processing of nociceptive data, to create sensory profiles by applying painful stimuli2828 Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tölle TR, Treede -DR, Beyer A, Binder A, Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Bötefür IC, Braune S, Flor H, Huge V, Klug R, Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Maihöfner C, Rolko C, Schaub C, Scherens A, Sprenger T, Valet M, Wasserka B. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006;123(3):231-43.. However, hypnosis does not specifically affect one kind of peripheral afferent nerve fiber but has an impact on the central processing of perception3838 Brunoni AR, Mofa AH, Fregni F, Palm U, Padberg F, Blumberger DM, Daskalakis ZJ, Bennabi D, Haffen E, Alonzo A, Loo CK. Transcranial direct current stimulation for acute major depressive episodes: meta-analysis of individual patient data. Br J Psychiatry. 2016;208(6):522-31., as previously related.

The literature demonstrates that pain reduction has been more effective in individuals considered highly susceptible to hypnosis1919 Benhaiem JM, Attal N, Chauvin M, Brasseur L, Bouhassira D. Local and remote effects of hypnotic suggestions of analgesia. Pain. 2001;89(2-3):167-73.,2020 Paoletti G, Varanini M, Balocchi R, Morizzo C, Palombo C, Santarcangelo EL. Cardiovascular and respiratory correlates of deep nociceptive stimulation, suggestions for analgesia, pain imagery and cognitive load as a function of hypnotizability. Brain Res Bull. 2010;82(1-2):65-73.. The results of this research agree with the literature since most subjects (n=19, 70.4%) presented moderate to high susceptibility scores spread across the different groups, responding to pain or analgesia in healthy individuals subjected to specific hypnotic suggestion.

The present study was able to demonstrate that specific hypnotic suggestion alters both mechanical and thermal exteroceptive sensitivity evaluated by QST, widely used to investigate somatosensory sensitivity, evaluating the functions of sensory nerve fbers A-δ and C of healthy volunteers. This finding is considered a great start in the search of how hypnosis could be useful in individuals with pain. The exact mechanisms by which hypnosis alters sensory functions are not yet fully understood and are part of the authors› future goals. Therefore, perception modification mainly for analgesia in healthy individuals broad out the idea to use hypnosis in refractory patients who will not answer appropriately in conventional treatment for chronic pain. Moreover, further investigations into these individuals would enlighten and enable its application in clinical practice.

CONCLUSION

Data presented herein demonstrate that specific hypnotic suggestions can modulate peripheral nociception in healthy subjects, revealing its modulatory effect both for pain and analgesia, in addition to enabling discussions for further studies on its clinical applicability.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank Professor Daniel Ciampi de Andrade from Departamento de Neurologia da Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo (FMUSP - Faculty of Medicine, Department of Neurology of University of São Paulo) who gave support for the development of this work. The authors would also like to thank Valquíria Aparecida Silva from Departamento de Neurologia da Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo (FMUSP - Faculty of Medicine, Department of Neurology of University of São Paulo) and Victhor

Teixeira de Oliveira from Departamento de Anatomia do Instituto de Ciências Biomédicas da Universidade de São Paulo (ICB/USP - Department of Anatomy, Institute of Biomedical Sciences of University of São Paulo) for their assistance in QST test and images. The authors would also like to thank Ricardo Galhardoni from Departamento de Neurologia da Faculdade de Medicina Universidade de São Paulo (FMUSP - Faculty of Medicine, Department of Neurology of University of São Paulo) and Elia Francesco Vedda from Departamento de Anatomia do Instituto de Ciências Biomédicas da Universidade de São Paulo (ICB/USP - Department of Anatomy, Institute of Biomedical Sciences of University of São Paulo) for their technical support in QST statistics. The authors would also like to thank Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP - São Paulo State Research Support Foundation) for the funding: VRSO (Grants 2016/10372-0; 2018/18483-1) and CSD (Grant 2018/14560-1) were supported by Fundação de Amparo à Pequisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP - São Paulo State Research Support Foundation). IPO (Grant 830928/1992-8) and BME (Grant 156313/2018-9) were supported by Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq - National Council for Scientific and Technological Development).

SUPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

1. Consort A Items to include when reporting a randomized trial in a journal or conference abstract
Item Description Reported on page number
Title Effects of specific hypnotic suggestions on mechanical and thermal sensitivity of healthy volunteers: randomized and double-blind study 1
Authors * Camila Squarzoni Dale (e-mail: camila.dale@usp.br) – corresponding author 1
Trial design N/A
Methods
Participants 27 volunteers were enrolled in this study. Inclusion criteria: 18-65 years and absence of complaints of pain or psychological disorders. Non-inclusion criteria: subjects with a history of major psychiatric disease, substance abuse, or those unable to understand the consent form, individuals complaining of pain, and severe systemic, metabolic, or neurological disease capable of influencing quantitative sensory testing (QST). 2,5
Interventions Specific hypnotic suggestions (hypnosis-induced pain and hypnosis-induced analgesia) 2,5
Objective To evaluate the mechanical and thermal nociception after the specific hypnotic suggestions technique in healthy volunteers. 2,5
Outcome specific hypnotic suggestions capable of alters the mechanical and thermal nociception in healthy subjects, revealing its modulatory effect both for pain and analgesia 2, 12-14, 16
Randomization

Computer-generated randomization (Excel software) was used to assign participant to three different groups:

a) sham group (no induction of hypnosis),

b) hypnosis-induced pain group,

c) hypnosis-induced analgesia group

2,5
Blinding (masking) the hypnotherapist was blinded to participant hypnosis susceptibility score, and the outcome assessor was blinded to group allocation. 2,5
Results
Numbers randomized

a) sham group (no induction of hypnosis) = 9

b) hypnosis-induced pain group = 11

c) hypnosis-induced analgesia group = 7

2,11
Recruitment advertisements posted in the university (USP) 2
Numbers analysed

a) sham group (no induction of hypnosis) = 9

b) hypnosis-induced pain group = 11

c) hypnosis-induced analgesia group = 7

2,11
Outcome

a) no significant changes were observed in the exteroceptive sensation of the sham group, even for warm or cold sensations,

b) hypnosis was able to induce mechanical hyperalgesia and decreased thermal pain thresholds in hypnosis-induced pain group,

c) hypnosis increased thermal pain thresholds in hypnosis-induced analgesia group.

12-16
Harms No
Conclusions specific hypnotic suggestions could alter the mechanical and thermal nociception in healthy subjects, revealing its modulatory effect both for pain and analgesia response. 16
Trial registration N/A
Funding Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP): [Grants 2016/10372-0; 2018/18483-1] and [Grant 2018/14560-1]. Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq): [Grant 830928/1992-8] and [Grant 156313/2018-9] 17
2 Tidier (Template for Intervention Description and Replication)

The TIDieR (Template for Intervention Description and Replication) Checklist*

Information to include when describing an intervention and the location of the information

Item number Item Where located **
Primary paper (page or appendix number) Other (details)
1.

BRIEF NAME

Specific Hypnotic Suggestion.

7
2.

WHY

Hypnosis is considered a psychological intervention to treat diseases such as depression, obesity, and phobias, and is one of the oldest forms of pain management.

4
3.

WHAT

The subject remained seated in a comfortable chair, at your disposal is a headphone and paper and pen to be used when necessary/requested.

4.

Before hypnosis starts the hypnotic susceptibility test was applied by hypnotherapist. The procedure was verbally standardized through a headphone containing four sensorial families: psycho-imaginary, psycho-conflictive, polyvalent, and sensorial. After 20 minutes of hypnotic induction, the presentation of 12 hypnotic suggestions (1. Hand lowering; 2. Moving hands together; 3. Mosquito hallucination; 4. Taste hallucination; 5. Arm rigidity; 6. Dream about hypnosis; 7. Arm immobilization; 8. Age regression; 9. Music hallucination (hear jingle bells); 10. Negative visual hallucination; 11. Posthypnotic suggestion (draw a doodle of a tree on the response booklet), and 12. Posthypnotic amnesia). All participants were subjected to the hypnotic susceptibility scale to be classified as low (0–3 points), moderate (4-8 points), or high (9-12 points) susceptibility to hypnosis according to their score.

After this, the hypnotic suggestion for pain or analgesia were applied. The hypnosis protocol consisted of two phases: induction and hypnotic suggestions. The standard hypnotic protocol began with an induction that is associated with breathing and relaxation, where subjects received suggestions to focus their attention on a single stimulus until reaching a trance state. This phase lasted around 10 minutes. After the induction, the hypnotic suggestions phase started, working on the imaginary, giving color, size, and shape to the sensation referred according to the hypnotic suggestion for pain and analgesia was given targeting the increase and decrease of the subject’s pain and controls over their sensations in those groups, rescuing and using an interpretation already known to the hypnotized person.

6-7
5.

WHO PROVIDED

Qualified hypnotherapist. Technical training and clinical experience were required.

7
6.

HOW

Hypnosis was delivery in a face-to-face format with the presence of a hypnotherapist, it was also provided individually.

7
7.

WHERE

Non sophisticated infrastructure was required, only a quiet room with a comfortable chair.

6-7
8.

WHEN and HOW MUCH

Hypnosis was applied just once, for 20 minutes divided in two phases: induction and hypnotic suggestion, 10 minutes each.

7
9.

TAILORING

The procedure was designed to be applied in a similar way to each volunteer. Except for the fact that the hypnotherapist would only find out in the time of application what kind of hypnotic suggestion he would

5
10.

MODIFICATIONS

No modifications were necessary.

11.

HOW WELL

The intervention adherence or fidelity was not assessed. All volunteers were eligible for the study.

5.9
12. The intervention adherence or fidelity was not assessed. All volunteers were eligible for the study. 5.9
  • ** Authors - use N/A if an item is not applicable for the intervention being described. Reviewers – use ‘?’ if information about the element is not reported/not sufficiently reported.
  • If the information is not provided in the primary paper, give details of where this information is available. This may include locations such as a published protocol or other published papers (provide citation details) or a website (provide the URL).
  • If completing the TIDieR checklist for a protocol, these items are not relevant to the protocol and cannot be described until the study is complete.
  • * We strongly recommend using this checklist in conjunction with the TIDieR guide (see BMJ 2014;348:g1687) which contains an explanation and elaboration for each item.
  • * The focus of TIDieR is on reporting details of the intervention elements (and where relevant, comparison elements) of a study. Other elements and methodological features of studies are covered by other reporting statements and checklists and have not been duplicated as part of the TIDieR checklist. When a randomized trial is being reported, the TIDieR checklist should be used in conjunction with the CONSORT statement (see www.consort-statement.org) as an extension of Item 5 of the CONSORT 2010 Statement. When a clinical trial protocol is being reported, the TIDieR checklist should be used in conjunction with the SPIRIT statement as an extension of Item 11 of the SPIRIT 2013 Statement (see www.spirit-statement.org). For alternate study designs, TIDieR can be used in conjunction with the appropriate checklist for that study design (see www.equator-network.org).
    • Sponsoring sources: VRSO (Grants 2016/10372-0; 2018/18483-1) and CSD (Grant 2018/14560-1) were supported by Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP - São Paulo State Research Support Foundation). IPO (Grant 830928/1992-8) and BME (Grant 156313/2018-9) were supported by Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq - National Council for Scientific and Technological Development).

    REFERENCES

    • 1
      Facco E, Casiglia E, Zanette G, Testoni I. On the way of liberation from suffering and pain: role of hypnosis in palliative care. Ann Palliat Med. 2018;7(1):63-74.
    • 2
      Jensen M P, Patterson DR. Hypnotic approaches for chronic pain management: clinical implications of recent research findings. Am Psychol. 2014;69(2):167-77.
    • 3
      Morone NE, Greco CM. Mind-body interventions for chronic pain in older adults: a structured review. Pain Med. 2007;8(4):359-75.
    • 4
      Bernardy K, Füber N, Klose P, Häuser W. Efficacy of hypnosis/guided imagery in fibromyalgia syndrome--a systematic review and meta-analysis of controlled trials. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2011;15;12:133.
    • 5
      Wetering EJ, Lemmens KM, Nieboer A P, Huijsman R. Cognitive and behavioral interventions for the management of chronic neuropathic pain in adults--a systematic review. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(7):670-81.
    • 6
      Tompson T, Terhune DB, Oram C, Sharangparni J, Rouf R, Solmi M, Veronese N, Stubbs B. The effectiveness of hypnosis for pain relief: A systematic review and meta-a-nalysis of 85 controlled experimental trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;99:298-310.
    • 7
      Milling LS, Valentine KE, LoStimolo LM, Nett AM, McCarley HS. Hypnosis and the Alleviation of Clinical Pain: A Comprehensive Meta-Analysis. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 2021;69(3):297-322.
    • 8
      Cardeña E. The phenomenology of deep hypnosis: quiescent and physically active. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 2005;53(1):37-59.
    • 9
      Cardeña E, Jönsson P, Terhune DB, Marcusson-Clavertz D. The neurophenomenology of neutral hypnosis. Cortex. 2013;49(2):375-85.
    • 10
      Kiernan BD, Dane JR, Phillips LH, Price DD. Hypnotic analgesia reduces R-III nociceptive reflex: further evidence concerning the multifactorial nature of hypnotic analgesia. Pain. 1995;60(1):39-47.
    • 11
      Castel A, Pérez M, Sala J, Padrol A, Rull M. Effect of hypnotic suggestion on fbromyalgic pain: comparison between hypnosis and relaxation. Eur J Pain. 2007;11(4):463-8.
    • 12
      Zachariae R, Andersen OK, Bjerring P, Jørgensen MM, Arendt-Nielsen L. Effects of an opioid antagonist on pain intensity and withdrawal reflexes during induction of hypnotic analgesia in high- and low-hypnotizable volunteers. Eur J Pain. 1998;2(1):25-34.
    • 13
      Facco E, Pasquali S, Zanette G, Casiglia E. (2013). Hypnosis as sole anaesthesia for skin tumour removal in a patient with multiple chemical sensitivity. Anaesthesia. 2013;68(9):961-5.
    • 14
      Provençal SC, Bond S, Rizkallah E, El-Baalbaki G. Hypnosis for burn wound care pain and anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Burns. 2018;44(8):1870-81.
    • 15
      Madden K, Middleton P, Cyna AM, Matthewson M, Jones L. Hypnosis for pain management during labour and childbirth. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016;(5):CD009356.
    • 16
      Bongartz W. German norms for the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1985;33(2):131-9.
    • 17
      Bowers KS. Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C: manual and response booklet. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1998;46(3):250-68.
    • 18
      Carli G, Manzoni D, Santarcangelo EL. Hypnotizability-related integration of perception and action. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 2008;25(7-8):1065-76.
    • 19
      Benhaiem JM, Attal N, Chauvin M, Brasseur L, Bouhassira D. Local and remote effects of hypnotic suggestions of analgesia. Pain. 2001;89(2-3):167-73.
    • 20
      Paoletti G, Varanini M, Balocchi R, Morizzo C, Palombo C, Santarcangelo EL. Cardiovascular and respiratory correlates of deep nociceptive stimulation, suggestions for analgesia, pain imagery and cognitive load as a function of hypnotizability. Brain Res Bull. 2010;82(1-2):65-73.
    • 21
      Gay MC, Philippot P, Luminet O. Differential effectiveness of psychological interventions for reducing osteoarthritis pain: a comparison of Erikson [correction of Erickson] hypnosis and Jacobson relaxation. Eur J Pain. 2002;6(1):1-16.
    • 22
      Arendt-Nielsen L, Yarnitsky D. Experimental and clinical applications of quantitative sensory testing applied to skin, muscles and viscera. J Pain. 2009;10(6):556-72.
    • 23
      Siviero M, Teixeira MJ, de Siqueira JT, Siqueira SR. Somesthetic, gustatory, olfactory function and salivary fow in patients with neuropathic trigeminal pain. Oral Dis. 2010;16(5):482-7.
    • 24
      Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor T F, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption--II. Addiction. 1993;88(6):791-804.
    • 25
      Castro MM, Quarantini L, Batista-Neves S, Kraychete DC, Daltro C, Miranda-Scippa A. Validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale in patients with chronic pain. Rev Bras Anestesiol. 2006;56(5):470-7.
    • 26
      Ferreira KA, Teixeira MJ, Mendonza TR, Cleeland CS. Validation of brief pain inventory to Brazilian patients with pain. Support Care Cancer. 2011;19(4):505-11.
    • 27
      Patterson DR, Jensen M P. Hypnosis and clinical pain. Psychol Bull. 2003;129(4):495-521.
    • 28
      Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tölle TR, Treede -DR, Beyer A, Binder A, Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Bötefür IC, Braune S, Flor H, Huge V, Klug R, Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Maihöfner C, Rolko C, Schaub C, Scherens A, Sprenger T, Valet M, Wasserka B. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006;123(3):231-43.
    • 29
      Mücke M, Cuhls H, Radbruch L, Baron R, Maier C, Tölle T, Treede RD, Rolke R. Quantitative sensory testing (QST). English version. Schmerz. 2021;35(Suppl 3)153-60.
    • 30
      de Melo DM, Barbosa AJ. (2015). O uso do Mini-Exame do Estado Mental em pesquisas com idosos no Brasil: uma revisão sistemática. Cien Saude Colet. 2015;20(12):3865-76.
    • 31
      Velasco R, Videla S, Villoria J, Ortiz E, Navarro X, Bruna J. Reliability and accuracy of quantitative sensory testing for oxaliplatin-induced neurotoxicity. Acta Neurol Scand. 2015;131(5):282-9.
    • 32
      Nash MR, Barnier AJ. (Eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research and Practice. New York: Oxford University Press. Reviewed by Eric K. Willmarth, PhD, Saybrook Graduate School and Research Center, San Francisco, CA. 2008.
    • 33
      Conversa G, Facco E, Leoni MLG, Buonocore M, Bagnasco R, Angelini L, Demartini L, Spiegel D. Quantitative Sensory Testing (QST) Estimation of regional cutaneous thermal sensitivity during waking state, neutral hypnosis, and temperature specific suggestions. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 2019;67(3):364-81.
    • 34
      Caratelli E, Menzocchi M, Carli G, Fontani G, Santarcangelo E. Is high hypnotizability a trouble in balance control? Eur J Appl Physiol. 2010;108(1):201-6.
    • 35
      Menzocchi M, Paoletti G, Carli G, Scattina E, Manzoni D, Santarcangelo EL. Hypnotizability -related effects of vestibular impairment on posture and locomotion. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 2010;58(3):329-44.
    • 36
      Montgomery GH, David D, Winkel G, Silverstein JH, Bovbjerg DH. The effectiveness of adjunctive hypnosis with surgical patients: a meta-analysis. Anesth Analg. 2002;94(6):1639-45.
    • 37
      Vanhaudenhuyse A, Boly M, Balteau E, Schnakers C, Moonen G, Luxen A, Lamy M, Degueldre C, Brichant J F, Maquet P, Laureys S, Faymonville ME. Pain and non-pain processing during hypnosis: a thulium-YAG event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2009;47(3):1047-54.
    • 38
      Brunoni AR, Mofa AH, Fregni F, Palm U, Padberg F, Blumberger DM, Daskalakis ZJ, Bennabi D, Haffen E, Alonzo A, Loo CK. Transcranial direct current stimulation for acute major depressive episodes: meta-analysis of individual patient data. Br J Psychiatry. 2016;208(6):522-31.
    • 39
      Kramer S, Zims R, Simang M, Rüger L, Irnich D. Hypnotic relaxation results in elevated thresholds of sensory detection but not of pain detection. BMC Complement Altern Med. Dec 2014;15;14:496.
    • 40
      Derbyshire S W, Whalley MG, Stenger VA, Oakley DA. Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain. Neuroimage. 2004;23(1):392-401.
    • 41
      Jiang H, White M P, Greicius MD, Waelde LC, Spiegel D. Brain activity and functional connectivity associated with hypnosis. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27(8):4083-93.
    • 42
      Baad-Hansen L, Abrahamsen R, Zachariae R, List T, Svensson P. Somatosensory sensitivity in patients with persistent idiopathic orofacial pain is associated with pain relief from hypnosis and relaxation. Clin J Pain. 2013;29(6):518-26.

    Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      16 Jan 2023
    • Date of issue
      Oct-Dec 2022

    History

    • Received
      22 Aug 2022
    • Accepted
      14 Nov 2022
    Sociedade Brasileira para o Estudo da Dor Av. Conselheiro Rodrigues Alves, 937 Cj2 - Vila Mariana, CEP: 04014-012, São Paulo, SP - Brasil, Telefones: , (55) 11 5904-2881/3959 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: dor@dor.org.br