Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

CUMULATIVE SEMANTIC INHIBITION IN SPANISH VERB PRODUCTION

ABSTRACT:

This paper reports an experiment designed to assess the occurrence of lexical interference in verb production in Spanish. Using the semantic competitors paradigm (HOWARD et al., 2006HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
), we show that the higher the number of verbs of the same category produced, the longer it takes to retrieve another verb of the same category in a picture naming task. According to our data, the production of a related verb delays the naming of a new member of the category by 20 ms., which means that, as is the case with nouns, semantically related verbs compete for selection. These results support the statement that, regardless their differences with respect to semantic representation and organization, verb and noun selection are guided by the same principles. Methodologically, our findings confirm that the paradigm is highly sensitive to semantic proximity effects, in terms of either categories or features. In light of these findings, we discuss the reasons why previous studies exploring semantic relatedness effects in verb production, most of them using the picture-word interference paradigm, have led to inconclusive and sometimes contradictory results.

KEYWORDS:
Semantic inhibition; Spoken word production; Verbs; Picture naming; Lexical interference

RESUMEN:

Este artículo presenta un experimento diseñado para evaluar la ocurrencia de interferencia léxica en la producción de verbos en español. Usando el paradigma de los competidores semánticos (HOWARD et al., 2006HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
), mostramos que cuanto mayor es el número de verbos de la misma categoría producidos, más tiempo lleva recuperar otro verbo de la misma categoría en una tarea de denominación de imágenes. Según nuestros datos, la producción de un verbo relacionado retrasa la denominación de un nuevo miembro de la categoría en 20 ms, lo que significa que, como es el caso para los sustantivos, los verbos relacionados semánticamente compiten por la selección. Estos resultados respaldan la propuesta de que, independientemente de sus diferencias con respecto a la representación y organización semántica, la selección de verbos y sustantivos se rige por los mismos principios. Metodológicamente, nuestros hallazgos confirman que el paradigma es altamente sensible a los efectos de proximidad semántica, en términos de categorías o rasgos. A la luz de estos hallazgos, discutimos las razones por las cuales los estudios previos que exploraron los efectos de la relación semántica en la producción de verbos, la mayoría de ellos utilizando el paradigma de interferencia entre palabras y dibujos, han llevado a resultados no concluyentes y a veces contradictorios.

PALABRAS CLAVE:
Inhibición semántica; Producción de palabras habladas; Verbos; Denominación de imágenes; Interferencia léxica

Introduction

The effects of semantic similarity on word production have been widely researched and are well documented in the literature. For example, it is well established that naming a picture presented simultaneously with a semantically related word (e.g. GLASER; DUNGELHOFF, 1984GLASER, W. R.; DÜNGELHOFF, F.-J. The time course of picture-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Washington, v.10, p.640–654, 1984.; SCHRIEFERS; MEYER; LEVELT, 1990SCHRIEFERS, H.; MEYER, A. S.; LEVELT, W. J. M. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.29, p.86-102, 1990. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90011-N
https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)900...
) or naming a picture that is semantically related to a word produced in response to a definition some trials earlier (e.g. WHEELDON; MONSELL, 1994WHEELDON, L. R.; MONSELL, S. Inhibition of spoken word production by priming a semantic competitor. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.33, p.332–356, 1994. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1016
https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1016...
) is slower as compared to an unrelated condition. This kind of effects has often been used to discuss semantic organisation principles and lexical selection processes. Nevertheless, as robust and useful as they may have been in the study of noun production, semantic effects have not been equally fruitful in the field of verb production. As a matter of fact, semantic effects in verbs have proved to be rather evasive so far: the studies are scarce and the results are inconclusive.

One reason why semantic interference effects are harder to find with verbs might be that verbs and nouns seem to be represented differently in the brain (e.g. SHAPIRO; MOO; CARAMAZZA, 2006SHAPIRO, K. A.; MOO, L. R.; CARAMAZZA, A. Cortical signatures of noun and verb production. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Washington, v.103, n.5, p.1644–1649, 2006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504142103
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504142103...
), which could lead to differences in the way they are accessed from the lexicon (e.g. HILLIS; TUFFIASH; CARAMAZZA, 2002HILLIS, A. E.; TUFFIASH, E.; CARAMAZZA, A. Modality-specific deterioration in naming verbs in nonfluent primary progressive aphasia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Cambridge, v.14, p.1099-1108, 2002. doi: 10.1162/089892902320474544
https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929023204745...
). Although the nature of grammatical category-specific effects is still debated, most of those reported in the literature seem to arise at a post-lexical level (morpho-syntax) (see VIGLIOCCO; VINSON; DRUKS et al, 2010VIGLIOCCO, G.; VINSON, D. P.; DRUKS, J. et al. Nouns and verbs in the brain: a review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, [s.l.], v.35, n.3, p.407-426, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010...
for a review). If grammatical class is not necessary as an organisational principle of lexical knowledge, there is no reason to expect differences in the way nouns and verbs are accessed from the lexicon.

A more plausible reason could be that the semantic relationships between verbs are more complex than those between nouns (e.g. verbs form less well defined categories or share features to a lesser extent or in more general ways; e.g. PLAUT, 1995PLAUT, D. C. Double dissociation without modularity: evidence from connectionist neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, Oxon, v.17, p.291-321, 1995. doi: 10.1080/01688639508405124
https://doi.org/10.1080/0168863950840512...
; VINSON; VIGLIOCCO, 2008VINSON, D.; VIGLIOCCO, G. Semantic feature production norms for a large set of objects and events. Behavior Research Methods, New York, v.40, n.1, p.183–190, 2008. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.1.183
https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.1.183...
; VINSON, 2009VINSON, D. P. Representing meaning: a feature-based model of object and action words. 2009. Thesis (Doctoral) - University College London, London, 2009.), making them harder to define because we do not have enough knowledge about the relevant criteria (e.g. SCHNUR; COSTA; CARAMAZZA, 2002SCHNUR, T.; COSTA, A.; CARAMAZZA, A. Verb production and the Semantic Interference Effect. Journal of Cognitive Science, Seoul, v.3, p.1-26, 2002.; TABOSSI; COLLINA, 2002TABOSSI, P.; COLLINA, S. The picture-word interference paradigm: conceptual effects in the production of verbs. Rivista di Linguistica, Ospedaletto, v.14, n.1, p.27–41, 2002.). In this case, the lack of an effect in some studies might simply be due to the fact that the paradigm used to test the occurrence of semantic interference in verbs might not have been sensitive enough to detect such effects because either representational complexity or descriptive difficulty. The aim of our study was to assess the presence of semantic interference in verb production by means of a paradigm that is known to be sensitive to a large spectrum of categorical or featural semantic relationships (e.g. ALARIO; MOSCOSO DEL PRADO MARTÍN, 2010ALARIO, F.-X.; MOSCOSO DEL PRADO MARTIN, F. On the origin of the cumulative semantic inhibition effect. Memory & Cognition, New York, v.38, p.57-66, 2010. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.1.57
https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.1.57...
), namely, the cumulative semantic interference paradigm (e.g. HOWARD et al., 2006HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
).

In the introduction we will first give a brief overview of the available evidence regarding semantic interference effects in verb production and discuss a potential problem arising in the use of picture-word interference effects as a proxy for lexical selection. Secondly, we will introduce the paradigm we relied on and the reasons why it is believed to index lexical selection. We will also present recent evidence showing that it is highly sensitive to effects of semantic interference. Finally, we will argue that, if nouns and verbs are accessed from the lexicon in similar ways, they should both elicit semantic interference effects in a sufficiently sensitive paradigm that taps into lexical selection.

Previous Research into Semantic Interference Effects in Verb Production

By far the most used paradigm to investigate the effects of semantic interference in verb production has been that of picture-word interference paradigm (e.g. GLASER; DUNGELHOFF, 1984GLASER, W. R.; DÜNGELHOFF, F.-J. The time course of picture-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Washington, v.10, p.640–654, 1984.; SCHRIEFERS; MEYER; LEVELT, 1990SCHRIEFERS, H.; MEYER, A. S.; LEVELT, W. J. M. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.29, p.86-102, 1990. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90011-N
https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)900...
). Participants have to name pictures while ignoring a simultaneously presented written word. The picture-word interference effect refers to the finding that participants are slower in naming the picture when the word is semantically related to the target picture than when there is no such relationship. This effect has been interpreted as reflecting competition between different lexical candidates: we know that whenever a speaker wants to express a concept through speech, semantically related concepts also become active and spread some activation to their lexical representations. The mechanism responsible for selecting the intended word is thought to operate taking account of the level of activation of all potential candidates, and eventually selecting the word with the highest level of activation (e.g. DELL, 1986DELL, G. S. A spreading activation model of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, Washington, v.93, n.3, p.83-321, 1986. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.2...
; LEVELT; ROELOFS; MEYER, 1999LEVELT, W. J. M.; ROELOFS, A.; MEYER, A. S. A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Cambridge, v.22, p.1-75, 1999. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99001776
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X9900177...
). Thus, if the activation level of competing candidates is boosted as in the picture-word interference paradigm, selection of the correct word is harder. While the picture-word interference effect is largely documented in noun production, in the case of verbs the results are less clear.

In a study using the picture-word interference paradigm, Roelofs (1993)ROELOFS, A. Testing a non-decompositional theory of lemma retrieval in speaking: Retrieval of verbs. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.47, p.59-87, 1993. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90062-Z
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)900...
obtained a robust semantic interference effect in bare verb naming in Dutch. Naming latencies for a verb were longer when the distracter was a semantically related verb than when it was an unrelated verb. In Roelofs's view, the competition between candidates is responsible for this effect, results suggesting that verb selection is guided by the same principles as the selection of nouns.

However, subsequent studies failed to obtain similar results and cast doubt on Roelofs's interpretation. In spite of their previous work replicating Roelofs's findings (TABOSSI; COLLINA, 1999TABOSSI, P.; COLLINA, S. The production of nouns and verbs. Psychonomics Society Bulletin, [s.l.], v.234, p.34–35, 1999.), Tabossi and Collina (2002)TABOSSI, P.; COLLINA, S. The picture-word interference paradigm: conceptual effects in the production of verbs. Rivista di Linguistica, Ospedaletto, v.14, n.1, p.27–41, 2002. failed to obtain interference effects in an experiment with Italian verbs. In other studies, semantic interference has been found only in some of the experimental conditions, and there does not seem to be systematicity with regard to what these conditions are. For instance, using an extension of the picture-word interference paradigm for sentence production in German, Schriefers, Teruel and Meinshausen (1998)SCHRIEFERS, H.; TERUEL, E.; MEINSHAUSEN, R. M. Producing simple sentences: results from picture–word interference experiments. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.39, p.609–632, 1998. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0026
https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0026...
found the semantic relatedness effect only for utterances with transitive verbs in initial position; no interference was observed for transitive verbs in final position or for intransitive verbs, regardless of their place in utterances. Additionally Schnur, Costa and Caramazza (2002)SCHNUR, T.; COSTA, A.; CARAMAZZA, A. Verb production and the Semantic Interference Effect. Journal of Cognitive Science, Seoul, v.3, p.1-26, 2002. found a semantic interference effect for intransitive verbs only.

However, there are a few studies that confirm the occurrence of semantic effects under strictly controlled conditions (BELKE; MEYER; DAMIAN, 2005BELKE, A. S.; MEYER, A. S.; DAMIAN, M. Refractory effects in picture naming as assessed in a semantic blocking paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, East Sussex, v.58, p.667-692, 2005. doi: 10.1080/02724980443000142
https://doi.org/10.1080/0272498044300014...
; VIGLIOCCO; LAUER et al., 2002VIGLIOCCO, G.; LAUER, M. et al. Semantic and syntactic forces in noun phrase production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Washington, v.28, p.46-58, 2002.; VIGLIOCCO; VINSON; LEWIS et al., 2004VIGLIOCCO, G.; VINSON, D. P.; LEWIS et al. Representing the meaning of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space (FUSS) hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, Maryland Heights, v.48, p.422–488, 2004. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2003.09.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2003....
). Vigliocco and Lauer et al. (2002)VIGLIOCCO, G.; LAUER, M. et al. Semantic and syntactic forces in noun phrase production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Washington, v.28, p.46-58, 2002., for example, tested the occurrence of interference effects in a cyclic naming task for both objects and actions, in blocks of semantically very similar, somewhat similar or dissimilar stimuli. They succeeded in finding semantic effects both for objects and actions modulated by the semantic distance between the exemplars. Based on this graded nature, it was suggested that semantic effects could be better accounted for in terms of featural overlapping. Also, Collina and Tabossi (2007)COLLINA, S.; TABOSSI P. Semantic interference effects in the production of verbs: the role of response set. The Mental Lexicon, Amsterdam, v.2, p.65-78, 2007. obtained semantic interference between verbs under the classical picture-word interference paradigm using the Italian translations of the stimuli employed by Roelofs (1993)ROELOFS, A. Testing a non-decompositional theory of lemma retrieval in speaking: Retrieval of verbs. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.47, p.59-87, 1993. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90062-Z
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)900...
. While these studies were able to discard some of the potential causes of the discrepant results (for instance response set; COLLINA; TABOSSI, 2007COLLINA, S.; TABOSSI P. Semantic interference effects in the production of verbs: the role of response set. The Mental Lexicon, Amsterdam, v.2, p.65-78, 2007.) and show the importance of the selection of materials (e.g. COLLINA; TABOSSI, 2007COLLINA, S.; TABOSSI P. Semantic interference effects in the production of verbs: the role of response set. The Mental Lexicon, Amsterdam, v.2, p.65-78, 2007.; VIGLIOCCO; LAUER et al., 2002VIGLIOCCO, G.; LAUER, M. et al. Semantic and syntactic forces in noun phrase production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Washington, v.28, p.46-58, 2002.), they did not settle the issue. Nonetheless, most researchers seem to concur with the conclusion reached by Schnur, Costa and Caramazza (2002)SCHNUR, T.; COSTA, A.; CARAMAZZA, A. Verb production and the Semantic Interference Effect. Journal of Cognitive Science, Seoul, v.3, p.1-26, 2002., that the inconsistency in semantic interference effects in verb production can be attributed to a series of factors, the most important being the complex semantic organisation of verbs, where categories can be difficult to distinguish and categorization levels cannot be established easily.

Aside from the inconclusive results, the picture-word interference paradigm is currently being subject to criticism by researchers who have argued that it does not tap into lexical processing but instead reflects the speed with which a response can be excluded from an output buffer in the parser (e.g. JANSSEN et al., 2008JANSSEN, N. et al. Semantic interference in a delayed naming task: evidence for the response exclusion hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, Washington, v.34, p.249-256, 2008. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.249
https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.2...
; MAHON et al., 2007MAHON, B.Z. et al. Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference & facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, Washington, v.33, p.503-535, 2007. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.503
https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.5...
). The reasoning behind this criticism is that written words are processed in a faster and more automatic manner by the linguistic system than pictures are. Thus, in the picture-word interference paradigm, although subjects are instructed to ignore the written words, they cannot help processing them. Given that the response required by the task is the name of the picture, the processing of the written word has to be detained at some point in order for the speaker not to articulate the wrong word. The locus of this response exclusion has been situated near the end of the speech production process, in a response buffer that takes into account all the relevant criteria for the desired response to be produced. One of these criteria is modality and another is semantics. Thus, in the case of written distracter words that are semantically related to the target, the distracter is harder to exclude because while the modality is clearly incompatible with the response, some aspects of the semantics not.

In sum, the studies investigating semantic interference effects in verb production have led to inconclusive and sometimes contradictory results, which is most likely due to the complex semantic organization of verbs or to descriptive difficulties. In addition, the paradigm used in the majority of these studies –picture-word interference– might not be informative about lexical processing. The issue thus calls for a new, fine-grained approach.

Cumulative Semantic Interference in Picture Naming

Howard and colleagues (2006)HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
introduced a new paradigm of semantic interference thought to reflect processes of lexical selection (e.g. COSTA; STRIJKERS et al., 2009COSTA, A.; STRIJKERS, K. et al. The time-course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Washington, v.106, p.21442-21446, 2009. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908921106
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908921106...
; HOWARD et al., 2006HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
; but see NAVARRETE; MAHON; CARAMAZZA, 2010NAVARRETE, E.; MAHON, B. Z.; CARAMAZZA, A. The cumulative semantic cost does not reflect lexical selection by competition. Acta Psychologica, Amsterdam, v.134, n.3, p.279-289, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02...
). In this study, participants had to name pictures of objects belonging to 24 different semantic categories that were intermixed in the stream of pictures. The crucial manipulation was the ordinal position in which a given picture was presented within its category. The authors observed a cumulative and linear increase in RTs with each ordinal position, meaning that every time participants named a picture belonging to the same semantic category as a previously named picture (e.g. naming “lion” after having named “tiger”) the response was a little bit slower (30 ms.). They argued that the cumulative semantic inhibitory effect is due to competition processes at the lexical level. When a picture is presented for naming, the semantic nodes corresponding to semantically related words also become active, although to a lesser degree, and spread activation to their lexical nodes (sharing of semantic activation). Those related lexical candidates that were named previously are given a head start in this process due to persisting activation (priming). Thus, assuming that the selection mechanism takes into account the level of activation of all active lexical candidates (selection by competition), the result is an increasingly slower process. Note that this effect cannot be accounted for in terms of response exclusion since a) only one stimulus is presented at a time; and b) the response exclusion hypothesis does not predict cumulative effects. This effect seems to be very sensitive to different strength in semantic relationships. The study of Howard et al. (2006)HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
included, as mentioned above, 24 different semantic categories. As highlighted by Alario and Moscoso del Prado Martín (2010)ALARIO, F.-X.; MOSCOSO DEL PRADO MARTIN, F. On the origin of the cumulative semantic inhibition effect. Memory & Cognition, New York, v.38, p.57-66, 2010. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.1.57
https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.1.57...
, there was considerable diversity in how these categories were defined, ranging from very general (e.g. furniture) to very specific (e.g. computer equipment, headgear). In an extended analysis of the data set of Howard and colleagues, Alario and Moscoso del Prado Martín found that there was systematic variability in the amount of inhibition between categories, but that all 24 categories included in the experiment contributed to the cumulative inhibition effect. So far, the paradigm has proven to be productive and sensitive enough to yield robust results in different domains of lexical processing research (see HARVEY; TRAUT; MIDDLETON, 2019HARVEY, D. Y.; TRAUT, H. J.; MIDDLETON, E. L. Semantic interference in speech error production in a randomised continuous naming task: evidence from aphasia. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, Oxon, v.34, n.1, p.69-86, 2019.) on aphasic word production; Oppenheim (2018)OPPENHEIM, G. M. The paca that roared: Immediate cumulative semantic interference among newly acquired words. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.177, p.21-29, 2018. on newly acquired words; Runnqvist et al. (2012)RUNNQVIST, E. et al. Cumulative semantic interference is blind to language: implications for models of bilingual speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.66, n.4, p.850-869, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.02.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.02.00...
on bilingual speech production, among others). Thus, this paradigm has the potential to solve the methodological difficulties (lack of sensitivity) and the theoretical problems (failure to reflect lexical processing) problems of previous research into semantic interference in verb production.

Is there Cumulative Semantic Interference in Verb Production?

In the context of the inconclusive and problematic results obtained in previous studies, the robustness of the evidence obtained for nouns with the cumulative semantic interference paradigm renders it interesting to explore the less well-known domain of verb production. This paper reports an experiment designed to test the occurrence of the effect obtained by Howard et al. (2006)HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
for verb production in Spanish. A positive result would allow us to conclude that a) the absence of an effect in previous studies is most likely related to the insufficient sensitivity of the experimental paradigm to effects in categories with more complex or less well defined structures; and b) the positive effect obtained in other studies using the picture-word interference paradigm can be replicated in a paradigm that is thought to tap into lexical selection and whose results cannot be explained in terms of response exclusion. This would mean that the selection process for verbs and nouns is similar.

Method

Participants

37 native Spanish speakers aged 21-40 (16 women, 21 men) drawn from students and staff of the School of Philosophy and Letters and the School of Science of the University of Buenos Aires volunteered as participants.

Materials

Seventy-one line drawings were selected (see Appendix 3 APPENDIX 3 Picture samples for samples). Twenty-five of the 71 belonged to 5 different categories: manner of motion, (animal) sound emission, (non agentive) substance emission, contact, and transfer,1 1 Categories were chosen following Levin (1993) and Levin and Rappaport (1995). The inclusion of verbs within each category was done using the semantic classification of the ADESSE database (Base de datos de verbos, alternancias de diátesis y esquemas sintáctico-semánticos del español [ADESSE]. Available at http://adesse.uvigo.es). A subjective rating was also performed in which 24 participants rated the semantic relationship between the words in each category through a Likert scale ranging from 1 to 7. Only items with an average equal to or above 6 were included. with 5 items in each category (see Appendix 1 APPENDIX 1 Target verbs by category (Acceptable alternatives) [English translations] Manner of movement: saltar [jump]; correr [run]; caminar [walk]; marchar [march]; nadar [swim]; Animal sound emission: mugir [moo]; ladrar [bark]; maullar [meow]; relinchar [neigh] rugir [roar] Non agentive substance emission: sangrar [bleed]; sudar (transpirar) [sweat]; llorar [cry]; babear [drool]; vomitar [vomit] Contact: golpear (pegar) [hit]; empujar [push]; pellizcar [pinch]; masajear [massage]; acariciar [caresse] Transfer: dar (entregar) [give]; pagar [pay]; regalar [give as a present]; robar [steal]; vender [sell] for the complete list). The fact that no further categories were taken into account was motivated by the scarcity of related items with a similar semantic behaviour. In addition, 40 fillers and 6 practice items were selected, bearing no semantic relations with the experimental verbs.

Design and Procedure

After the first 6 practice items, participants were presented with a sequence of 65 pictures for naming. Following the procedure of Howard et al. (2006)HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
, five exemplars from each of the categories under study were embedded within the sequence. Pictures belonging to the same category were separated by a lag of 2, 4, 6, or 8 intervening items; each interval was present once for every category. Hence, 23 items intervened between the first and last items in each category.

Thirty-seven different lists were created with the following constraints. Items intervening between members of a category might either be drawn from other experimental categories or be fillers. The position of filler items and the order of the categories in the sequence of 65 items were randomly assigned, except that only fillers could occur in the first five positions of the sequence.

Lag order was pseudorandomised, and effort was taken to ensure that each of the 24 possible lag orders occurred a similar number of times across all lists. Likewise, different lag orders appeared in a given ordinal position (2 to 5) a similar number of times. Importantly, in order to avoid item-specific effects, we ensured that every experimental item appeared a similar number of times (between 7 and 8 times for all lists) in each ordinal position within its category.

Stimuli were black-and-white line drawings sized 300 x 300 pixels and presented centred on a 1024x768 pixel screen in 32 colours. The experiment was run in DMDX (FORSTER; FORSTER, 2003FORSTER, K. I.; FORSTER, J. C. DMdX: a windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods Instruments and Computers, New York, v.35, p.116–124, 2003.). Each picture was preceded by a visual cue (an asterisk) for 500 ms, followed by a blank screen for 250 ms. RTs were recorded by DMDX's voice key from the onset of the picture. The picture remained on the screen until detection of a response or for a maximum of 2000 ms. and was followed by a new blank screen for 500 ms. The DMDX-recorded RTs were checked for accuracy and timing (properly triggered voice key) using CheckVocal (PROTOPAPAS, 2007PROTOPAPAS, A. Checkvocal: a program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMdX. Behavior Research Methods, New York, v.39, n.4, p.859–862, 2007.).

Subjects were instructed to name each picture as rapidly and accurately as possible, describing what the actor shown was doing. Thus, they had to produce a verb. There was no familiarization phase, nor feedback throughout the experimental session, after the practice set. During this training, feedback was given if required to achieve the expected answer. The experiment lasted approximately 12 minutes.

Data Analysis

Two types of responses were excluded from the analysis: naming errors and omissions. Semantically non related verbs and words from other lexical classes were considered naming errors. Latencies of more than 2000 ms. (responses initiated after the picture disappeared) were considered omissions. Acceptable alternatives (e.g. “entregar” instead of “dar;” see Appendix 1 APPENDIX 1 Target verbs by category (Acceptable alternatives) [English translations] Manner of movement: saltar [jump]; correr [run]; caminar [walk]; marchar [march]; nadar [swim]; Animal sound emission: mugir [moo]; ladrar [bark]; maullar [meow]; relinchar [neigh] rugir [roar] Non agentive substance emission: sangrar [bleed]; sudar (transpirar) [sweat]; llorar [cry]; babear [drool]; vomitar [vomit] Contact: golpear (pegar) [hit]; empujar [push]; pellizcar [pinch]; masajear [massage]; acariciar [caresse] Transfer: dar (entregar) [give]; pagar [pay]; regalar [give as a present]; robar [steal]; vender [sell] for the full list of items and acceptable alternatives) were considered right answers and included in the analysis.

Data were analysed for the number of unrelated items between the occurrence of two items in a category (lag) and for its ordinal position within the category. Following the protocol of Howard et al. (2006)HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
, separate analyses were carried out treating subjects and categories as random factors, leading to F1 and F2 statistics.

Results

In analysing the data, naming errors (7.46%) and omissions (6.38%) were excluded. In total, excluded data accounted for 13.84% responses. Including acceptable alternatives, the analysis was performed over 797 data points. For each removed data point, we corrected the position of the other items corresponding to the same category, so that the analysis was performed taking into account the actual position of each item within its category. Naming errors consisted mainly of lexical class substitutions (“a baby”; “a throw”; “hangover”), semantically non-related verbs (“have fun” for walk; sometimes due to visual errors: “clean” for bleed) or comments on the picture (“he's having a bad time”). Errors and omissions were uniformly distributed across categories.

The two effects we wanted to identify on response times were (a) that of the ordinal position within a category and (b) that of the lag between members of the same category.

The experiment's design minimised but did not eliminate the confound between the ordinal position of an item within a category and its serial position in the experiment. Due to the total number of stimuli and categories and given that the last category had to begin at least 23 positions before the end of the experiment, the highest positions tended to be concentrated at the end of the experimental lists. In order to neutralise any possible contribution of serial position, we decided to adjust subjects’ RTs for any linear changes in RT (either speeding up or slowing down) over the course of the experiment. All analyses were conducted on both linearly-adjusted and non-adjusted RTs.

First of all, we quantified absolute position effects within the experiment, making a linear regression of RTs with respect to position. The resulting gradient was 0.48 (-0.7765, 1.7457), F = 0.5689, p = .4509. In order to separate both effects, RTs were adjusted in terms of serial position. This adjustment should therefore tend to reduce position effect within a category. Table 1 shows latencies for each ordinal position and lag, for both uncorrected and corrected data.

Table 1
Naming Reaction Times, by Ordinal Position within the Category and Lag

Analysing uncorrected and corrected RTs for linear changes, we studied lag and ordinal position effects on naming latencies. Table 1 shows how mean RTs increased monotonically as a function of ordinal position (column means) but were unaffected by lag (row means). This holds for both the uncorrected and the corrected data. As is clear from Figure 1 (A), the analysis of uncorrected data showed an effect of ordinal position, even if it was significant only in the by subjects analysis (by subjects, gradient = 25 (9.33,40.6), F1(1,184) = 9.9166, p = .0019, MSE = 23264.49; by categories, gradient = 22.1 (-9.99,54.2), F2(1,24) = 2.0305, p = .1676, MSE = 12053.79), which holds for corrected data as well (by subjects, gradient = 22 (6.34,37.7), F1(1,184) = 7.6796, p = .0062, MSE = 23354.38; by categories, gradient = 19.2 (-13,51.3), F2(1,24) = 1.5220, p = .2298, MSE = 12056.95), as is evident in Figure 1 (B). On the other hand, the analysis showed no significant lag effect (uncorrected data: by subjects, gradient = 5.69 (-6.38,17.8), F1(1,147) = 0.8674, p = .3532, MSE = 27612.25; by categories, gradient = 3.61 (-19.7,26.9), F2(1,19) = 0.1056, p = .7490, MSE = 12313.02; corrected data: by subjects, gradient = 5.38 (-6.69,17.4), F1(1,147) = 0.7757, p =.3799, MSE = 27599.13; by categories, gradient = 3.31 (-20,26.6), F2(1,19) = 0.0888, p =.7691, MSE = 12328.11).

Figure 1
Figure A shows ordinal position effect on latency for uncorrected data for linear changes. Figure B shows ordinal position effect on latency for corrected data for linear changes. Error bars correspond to the standard error.

In sum, our results replicate Howard et al.'s findings (2006) for nouns, showing that the higher the number of verbs of the same category produced previously, the longer it takes to retrieve another verb of the same category in a picture naming task, irrespective of the number of intervening unrelated items (lag).

Discussion

In this study we wanted to assess the presence of semantic interference in verb production. We hypothesized that previous inconsistencies in such effects could be explained in term of either a more complex semantic organization of verbs as compared to nouns or the difficulty in defining semantic categories of verbs accurately, complicating the detection of semantic interference. We conducted an experiment within a paradigm known to be sensitive to a large spectrum of categorical or featural semantic relationships: the cumulative semantic interference paradigm introduced by Howard and colleagues (2006)HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
. In this experiment, participants had to produce verbs conjugated in the third person present in response to pictures representing actions. Within the stream of pictures three variables were manipulated: a) semantic category membership; b) ordinal position of a given member within its category; and c) lag between ordinal positions.

Due to the numerous difficulties that verb studies had encountered in reproducing the effects verified in nouns, the design of our study favoured, in the selection of materials, the control of the variables that could be relevant. In addition to belonging to the same category, defined according to the ADESSE base, it was sought, based on a subjective assessment survey, that the selected stimuli had a maximum semantic proximity. Additionally, we controlled for the level of categorization, that is to say that none of the members of the category could be considered superordinate or subordinate with respect to another (COSTA; MAHON et al., 2003COSTA, A.; MAHON, B. et al. Level of categorization effect: A novel effect in the picture-word interference paradigm. Language and Cognitive Processes, Oxon, v.18, n.2, p.205–233, 2003. doi: 10.1080/01690960143000524.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0169096014300052...
). In addition, the maximum distance between categories was sought, in order to avoid overlaps (that is, that an item of a category could be a competitor with respect to another of a different category). We also controlled for the syntactic behaviour. Although they are largely predictable from semantic properties, we independently verified that the verbs in a category showed equivalent selectional properties. These restrictions allowed to build a very limited set of materials. Replicating previous studies, we found that, within a given category, naming latencies increased 20 ms. with each ordinal position, and this effect was independent of the number of intervening trials (lag). The magnitude reported in the literature regarding nouns is around 30 ms. per item.

Our experiment is, so far, the first to verify cumulative effects of semantic competitors for verbs. It is, on the other hand, one of the firsts to confirm the validity of the effect in Spanish, regardless of category.2 2 The effect of cumulative semantic inhibition for nouns in Spanish was proven by a study of our authorship. See Sevilla, Martínez-Cuitiño & Shalóm (2008). García Castro et al. (2008) also encountered priming effect of semantic competitors for nouns in Spanish and Catalan. The fact that verbs are susceptible to interference from semantic competitors thus constitutes strong evidence that words in this class are selected under competition.

The occurrence of this cumulative interference effect due to the previous recovery of other semantically related words can only be explained under the assumption that three properties are essential to the speech production system (HOWARD et al., 2006HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
). First, there is coactivation at the semantic level: the semantic nodes corresponding to words related to the target you are trying to produce are activated simultaneously, although to a lesser extent than the target. Secondly, there is priming: the activation of a produced item is sustained over time, even if other words are selected during that period. Finally, there is competition in the system: the activation of related candidates affects the subsequent selection of a target making it slower and, predictably, more prone to errors. In other words, the results of Howard et al. (2006)HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
, later confirmed by several works, including the one presented here, demonstrated that the semantic inhibition effect described by Wheeldon and Monsell (1994)WHEELDON, L. R.; MONSELL, S. Inhibition of spoken word production by priming a semantic competitor. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.33, p.332–356, 1994. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1016
https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1016...
is a consequence of competition. It is also a uniformly accepted conclusion from these studies that competence is the result of the facilitation (that is, the sustained activation) of representations of semantically related words that have been accessed before.

Thus, at the theoretical level, the most obvious implication of our results is that lexical selection processes for verbs and nouns seem to obey similar principles. Although this might not be that surprising, the main contribution of our study is that it clears the doubts cast by the inconclusive data from previous studies.

At the methodological level, the fact that we found an effect of semantic interference in an experiment including a quite diverse set of verbs provides additional evidence that the cumulative semantic interference paradigm is highly sensitive to relationships between semantic features or categories (e.g. ALARIO; MOSCOSO DEL PRADO MARTÍN, 2010ALARIO, F.-X.; MOSCOSO DEL PRADO MARTIN, F. On the origin of the cumulative semantic inhibition effect. Memory & Cognition, New York, v.38, p.57-66, 2010. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.1.57
https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.1.57...
). In light of these results, caution should be exercised when drawing conclusions from the null results stemming from other paradigms that are less sensitive to such relationships. This also holds true for those null results obtained in the context of positive results we mentioned in the introduction (e.g. effects for transitive or intransitive verbs only; SCHNUR; COSTA; CARAMAZZA, 2002SCHNUR, T.; COSTA, A.; CARAMAZZA, A. Verb production and the Semantic Interference Effect. Journal of Cognitive Science, Seoul, v.3, p.1-26, 2002.; SCHRIEFERS; TERUEL; MEINSHAUSEN, 1998SCHRIEFERS, H.; TERUEL, E.; MEINSHAUSEN, R. M. Producing simple sentences: results from picture–word interference experiments. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.39, p.609–632, 1998. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0026
https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0026...
). As shown by Alario and Moscoso del Prado Martín (2010)ALARIO, F.-X.; MOSCOSO DEL PRADO MARTIN, F. On the origin of the cumulative semantic inhibition effect. Memory & Cognition, New York, v.38, p.57-66, 2010. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.1.57
https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.1.57...
, it is the case that, depending on the definition of a category or the strength of the relationship between its members, there is systematic variability in the amount of semantic interference. Thus, it is possible that the effects of categories with a small amount of inhibition become invisible with a less sensitive manipulation. Given the difficulty in defining categories in the domain of verbs –be it descriptive or due to a complex representational structure–, this variability could account for the inconsistent results for semantic interference effects in verb production.

Although it appears that the cumulative semantic interference paradigm is more suitable than the picture-word interference paradigm to test effects of semantic relatedness, it is worth to note that both paradigms quite consistently elicit the same pattern of results. Thus, even though these effects could have two independent sources leading to a similar behaviour, it seems more parsimonious to assume that previous positive results from the picture word interference paradigm actually tell us something about lexical processing and not only about response exclusion.

Following Howard's et al. (2006)HOWARD, D. et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005...
, our study uses semantic inhibition as a proxy to understand lexical selection mechanisms, and it does not commit to a theory about how conceptual and semantic knowledge about verbs is organized. In fact, although lexical access is similar, it may still be the case that lexico-semantic representations or the relationship between them is different for verbs and nouns. As we pointed out, although the effect of cumulative interference is addressed here as it indicates the selection under competition at the lexical level, in the interpretation of the results it is necessary to contemplate that the inhibition originates in the first instance in the co-activation of semantic related items in the conceptual level. In this sense, when weighing the results with respect to those of previous studies, carried out with nouns, the potential existence of differences in the conceptual knowledge of events and objects must be taken into account.

There are many reasons why nouns and verbs may differ regarding their lexico-semantic representation, two of them being the level of concreteness (verbs tend to be more abstract than nouns; e.g. VINSON; VIGLIOCCO, 2008VINSON, D.; VIGLIOCCO, G. Semantic feature production norms for a large set of objects and events. Behavior Research Methods, New York, v.40, n.1, p.183–190, 2008. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.1.183
https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.1.183...
) and the level of individuation (while most nouns are separable entities, verbs are usually not; e.g. SHAPIRO; MOO; CARAMAZZA, 2006SHAPIRO, K. A.; MOO, L. R.; CARAMAZZA, A. Cortical signatures of noun and verb production. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Washington, v.103, n.5, p.1644–1649, 2006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504142103
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504142103...
). This could presumably lead to more diffuse categories or weaker links between features.

However, it is equally plausible that the semantic complexity of verbs is only descriptive in nature, which does not preclude representational differences. For example, it might be the case that verbs are related in terms of motor aspects whereas relations between nouns are mainly based on visual features (e.g. KHADER et al., 2010KHADER, PH. et al. Neural correlates of generating visual nouns and motor verbs in a minimal phrase context. Brain Research, Amsterdam, v.1318, p.122-132, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.082
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009....
). In this scenario, the complexity of both categories could be similar, but the relevant criteria for defining semantic relationships would be different. Future research should aim at elucidating whether the semantic complexity attributed to verbs is a real psychological phenomenon or a descriptive difficulty. A highly sensitive paradigm like the cumulative semantic interference paradigm could offer the necessary tools to test these hypotheses.

Conclusion

In this paper we have shown that semantic interference holds in verb production using the paradigm of competing candidates. The results become particularly relevant in a context where experimental evidence for lexical selection in this word class has not been consistent so far.

Methodologically, our findings confirm that the paradigm is highly sensitive to semantic proximity effects, in terms of either categories or features. In a broader sense, they shed light on previous results obtained with the picture-word interference paradigm, suggesting that, even if they are less clear, they still say something about lexical selection processes. Theoretically, our data support the statement that verb and noun selection are guided by the same principles. More generally, they contribute to prove the uniformity of processing principles across word classes. This does not mean, however, that lexical-semantic representations of verbs and nouns are not different and have the same semantic organisation. Further studies are needed to determine the nature of the criteria governing semantic relationships between verbs that make the effect of interference between candidates more difficult to observe. Thus, cumulative inhibition seems to be a promising field for further exploration.

Acknowledgement

This research was supported by three grants from the Spanish Government (SEJ 2005/SEJ2005-01223/CONSOLIDER-INGENIO) and by grants PSIC and PSI2008-00464/PSIC, as well as by UBACyT F436, from the University of Buenos Aires. Yamila Sevilla and Diego E. Shalóm were supported by the National Research Council (Conicet), Argentina. Elin Runnqvist was supported by a fellowship from the Catalan Government (FI).

APPENDIX 1 Target verbs by category (Acceptable alternatives) [English translations]

  1. Manner of movement: saltar [jump]; correr [run]; caminar [walk]; marchar [march]; nadar [swim];

  2. Animal sound emission: mugir [moo]; ladrar [bark]; maullar [meow]; relinchar [neigh] rugir [roar]

  3. Non agentive substance emission: sangrar [bleed]; sudar (transpirar) [sweat]; llorar [cry]; babear [drool]; vomitar [vomit]

  4. Contact: golpear (pegar) [hit]; empujar [push]; pellizcar [pinch]; masajear [massage]; acariciar [caresse]

  5. Transfer: dar (entregar) [give]; pagar [pay]; regalar [give as a present]; robar [steal]; vender [sell]

APPENDIX 2 Fillers and practice ítems (Acceptable alternatives) [English translations]

Pescar [fish]; plantar [plant]; abrir [open]; tirar [throw]; exprimir [squeeze]; jugar [play]; construir [build]; rastrillar [rake]; dibujar [draw]; trepar [climb]; coser [sew]; comer [eat]; arreglar [fix]; barrer [sweep]; planchar [iron]; cocinar [cook]; pintar [paint]; llevar [bring]; encender [light]; pelar [peel]; fumar [smoke]; regar [water]; tejer [knit]; atar [tie]; rezar [pray]; servir [serve]; escribir [write]; borrar [delete]; arrestar (apresar) [arrest]; subir [go up]; engordar [fatten]; florecer [flourish]; dormir [sleep]; caer [fall]; guiñar [blink]; beber [drink]; cavar [dig]; cortar [cut]; leer [read]; perseguir [chase]; reír [laugh]; aplaudir [clap]; soñar [dream]; plegar (doblar) [fold]; disparar [shoot]; saludar [greet].

APPENDIX 3 Picture samples

REFERENCIAS

  • ALARIO, F.-X.; MOSCOSO DEL PRADO MARTIN, F. On the origin of the cumulative semantic inhibition effect. Memory & Cognition, New York, v.38, p.57-66, 2010. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.1.57
    » https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.1.57
  • BELKE, A. S.; MEYER, A. S.; DAMIAN, M. Refractory effects in picture naming as assessed in a semantic blocking paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, East Sussex, v.58, p.667-692, 2005. doi: 10.1080/02724980443000142
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000142
  • COLLINA, S.; TABOSSI P. Semantic interference effects in the production of verbs: the role of response set. The Mental Lexicon, Amsterdam, v.2, p.65-78, 2007.
  • COSTA, A.; MAHON, B. et al. Level of categorization effect: A novel effect in the picture-word interference paradigm. Language and Cognitive Processes, Oxon, v.18, n.2, p.205–233, 2003. doi: 10.1080/01690960143000524.
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960143000524
  • COSTA, A.; STRIJKERS, K. et al The time-course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Washington, v.106, p.21442-21446, 2009. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908921106
    » https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908921106
  • DELL, G. S. A spreading activation model of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, Washington, v.93, n.3, p.83-321, 1986. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
  • FORSTER, K. I.; FORSTER, J. C. DMdX: a windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods Instruments and Computers, New York, v.35, p.116–124, 2003.
  • GLASER, W. R.; DÜNGELHOFF, F.-J. The time course of picture-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Washington, v.10, p.640–654, 1984.
  • HARVEY, D. Y.; TRAUT, H. J.; MIDDLETON, E. L. Semantic interference in speech error production in a randomised continuous naming task: evidence from aphasia. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, Oxon, v.34, n.1, p.69-86, 2019.
  • HILLIS, A. E.; TUFFIASH, E.; CARAMAZZA, A. Modality-specific deterioration in naming verbs in nonfluent primary progressive aphasia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Cambridge, v.14, p.1099-1108, 2002. doi: 10.1162/089892902320474544
    » https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902320474544
  • HOWARD, D. et al Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.100, n.3, p.464-482, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
  • JANSSEN, N. et al Semantic interference in a delayed naming task: evidence for the response exclusion hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, Washington, v.34, p.249-256, 2008. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.249
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.249
  • KHADER, PH. et al Neural correlates of generating visual nouns and motor verbs in a minimal phrase context. Brain Research, Amsterdam, v.1318, p.122-132, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.082
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.082
  • LEVELT, W. J. M.; ROELOFS, A.; MEYER, A. S. A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Cambridge, v.22, p.1-75, 1999. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99001776
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99001776
  • LEVIN, B. Verb classes and alternations Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1993.
  • LEVIN, B.; RAPPAPORT, M. Unaccusativity Boston: MIT Press, 1995.
  • MAHON, B.Z. et al Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference & facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, Washington, v.33, p.503-535, 2007. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.503
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.503
  • NAVARRETE, E.; MAHON, B. Z.; CARAMAZZA, A. The cumulative semantic cost does not reflect lexical selection by competition. Acta Psychologica, Amsterdam, v.134, n.3, p.279-289, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.009
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.009
  • OPPENHEIM, G. M. The paca that roared: Immediate cumulative semantic interference among newly acquired words. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.177, p.21-29, 2018.
  • PLAUT, D. C. Double dissociation without modularity: evidence from connectionist neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, Oxon, v.17, p.291-321, 1995. doi: 10.1080/01688639508405124
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639508405124
  • PROTOPAPAS, A. Checkvocal: a program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMdX. Behavior Research Methods, New York, v.39, n.4, p.859–862, 2007.
  • ROELOFS, A. Testing a non-decompositional theory of lemma retrieval in speaking: Retrieval of verbs. Cognition, Amsterdam, v.47, p.59-87, 1993. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90062-Z
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90062-Z
  • RUNNQVIST, E. et al Cumulative semantic interference is blind to language: implications for models of bilingual speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.66, n.4, p.850-869, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.02.007
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.02.007
  • SHAPIRO, K. A.; MOO, L. R.; CARAMAZZA, A. Cortical signatures of noun and verb production. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Washington, v.103, n.5, p.1644–1649, 2006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504142103
    » https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504142103
  • SCHNUR, T.; COSTA, A.; CARAMAZZA, A. Verb production and the Semantic Interference Effect. Journal of Cognitive Science, Seoul, v.3, p.1-26, 2002.
  • SCHRIEFERS, H.; MEYER, A. S.; LEVELT, W. J. M. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.29, p.86-102, 1990. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90011-N
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90011-N
  • SCHRIEFERS, H.; TERUEL, E.; MEINSHAUSEN, R. M. Producing simple sentences: results from picture–word interference experiments. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.39, p.609–632, 1998. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0026
    » https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0026
  • TABOSSI, P.; COLLINA, S. The production of nouns and verbs. Psychonomics Society Bulletin, [s.l.], v.234, p.34–35, 1999.
  • TABOSSI, P.; COLLINA, S. The picture-word interference paradigm: conceptual effects in the production of verbs. Rivista di Linguistica, Ospedaletto, v.14, n.1, p.27–41, 2002.
  • VIGLIOCCO, G.; LAUER, M. et al Semantic and syntactic forces in noun phrase production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Washington, v.28, p.46-58, 2002.
  • VIGLIOCCO, G.; VINSON, D. P.; LEWIS et al Representing the meaning of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space (FUSS) hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, Maryland Heights, v.48, p.422–488, 2004. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2003.09.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2003.09.001
  • VIGLIOCCO, G.; VINSON, D. P.; DRUKS, J. et al Nouns and verbs in the brain: a review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, [s.l.], v.35, n.3, p.407-426, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007
  • VINSON, D. P. Representing meaning: a feature-based model of object and action words. 2009. Thesis (Doctoral) - University College London, London, 2009.
  • VINSON, D.; VIGLIOCCO, G. Semantic feature production norms for a large set of objects and events. Behavior Research Methods, New York, v.40, n.1, p.183–190, 2008. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.1.183
    » https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.1.183
  • WHEELDON, L. R.; MONSELL, S. Inhibition of spoken word production by priming a semantic competitor. Journal of Memory and Language, New York, v.33, p.332–356, 1994. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1016
    » https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1016
  • 1
    Categories were chosen following Levin (1993)LEVIN, B. Verb classes and alternations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1993. and Levin and Rappaport (1995)LEVIN, B.; RAPPAPORT, M. Unaccusativity. Boston: MIT Press, 1995.. The inclusion of verbs within each category was done using the semantic classification of the ADESSE database (Base de datos de verbos, alternancias de diátesis y esquemas sintáctico-semánticos del español [ADESSE]. Available at http://adesse.uvigo.es). A subjective rating was also performed in which 24 participants rated the semantic relationship between the words in each category through a Likert scale ranging from 1 to 7. Only items with an average equal to or above 6 were included.
  • 2
    The effect of cumulative semantic inhibition for nouns in Spanish was proven by a study of our authorship. See Sevilla, Martínez-Cuitiño & Shalóm (2008). García Castro et al. (2008) also encountered priming effect of semantic competitors for nouns in Spanish and Catalan.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    09 Nov 2020
  • Date of issue
    2020

History

  • Received
    29 Jan 2019
  • Accepted
    07 Oct 2019
Universidade Estadual Paulista Júlio de Mesquita Filho Rua Quirino de Andrade, 215, 01049-010 São Paulo - SP, Tel. (55 11) 5627-0233 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: alfa@unesp.br