Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The most frequently used tests for assessing executive functions in aging

Os testes neuropsicológicos mais utilizados para avaliar funções executivas no envelhecimento

Abstracts

There are numerous neuropsychological tests for assessing executive functions in aging, which vary according to the different domains assessed.

OBJECTIVE:

To present a systematic review of the most frequently used instruments for assessing executive functions in older adults with different educational levels in clinical and experimental research.

METHODS:

We searched for articles published in the last five years, using the PubMed database with the following terms: "neuropsychological tests", "executive functions", and "mild cognitive impairment". There was no language restriction.

RESULTS:

25 articles fulfilled all the inclusion criteria. The seven neuropsychological tests most frequently used to evaluate executive functions in aging were: [1] Trail Making Test (TMT) Form B; [2] Verbal Fluency Test (VFT) - F, A and S; [3] VFT Animals category; [4] Clock Drawing Test (CDT); [5] Digits Forward and Backward subtests (WAIS-R or WAIS-III); [6] Stroop Test; and [7] Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and its variants. The domains of executive functions most frequently assessed were: mental flexibility, verbal fluency, planning, working memory, and inhibitory control.

CONCLUSION:

The study identified the tests and domains of executive functions most frequently used in the last five years by research groups worldwide to evaluate older adults. These results can direct future research and help build evaluation protocols for assessing executive functions, taking into account the different educational levels and socio-demographic profiles of older adults in Brazil.

review; mild cognitive impairment; dementia; older adults; executive functions; neuropsychological tests


Os testes neuropsicológicos que avaliam as funções executivas no envelhecimento são muitos e variam de acordo com os diferentes domínios.

OBJETIVO:

Apresentar uma revisão sistemática dos instrumentos mais utilizados para avaliar as funções executivas em idosos com diferentes níveis de escolaridade em pesquisas clínicas e experimentais.

MÉTODOS:

Foram pesquisados os artigos publicados nos últimos cinco anos, utilizando a base de dados PubMed com os termos: "neuropsychological tests", "executive functions" e "mild cognitive impairment", sem restrição de idioma.

RESULTADOS:

25 preencheram todos os critérios de inclusão. Os sete testes neuropsicológicos mais utilizados para avaliar funções executivas em idosos foram: [1] o Teste de Trilhas (TMT) Forma B; [2] Teste de Fluência Verbal (TFV) fonológica - F, A e S; [3] TFV categoria Animais; [4] Teste Desenho do Relógio (TDR); [5] Subteste Dígitos (WAIS-R ou WAIS-III); [6] Teste Stroop e [7] o Teste Wisconsin de Classificação de Cartas (WCST) e suas variações. Os domínios das funções executivas mais escolhidos pelos pesquisadores para serem avaliados foram: flexibilidade mental, fluência verbal, planejamento, memória operacional e controle inibitório.

CONCLUSÃO:

O presente estudo identificou os testes e os domínios de funções executivas mais utilizados nos últimos cinco anos, pelos principais grupos de pesquisa, para avaliar idosos. Os resultados encontrados podem direcionar pesquisas futuras e auxiliar a criação de protocolos de avaliação das funções executivas adequados aos diferentes níveis de educação e aos diferentes perfis sociodemográficos dos idosos do Brasil.

Revisão; comprometimento cognitivo leve; demência; idosos; funções executivas; testes neuropsicológicos


INTRODUCTION

EFs are complex cognitive abilities that enable the identification of goals, mental planning, behavior organization, and planning actions to achieve these goals.22. Diamond A. Executive Functions. Annu Rev Psychol 2013;64:135-68. , 33. Kluwe-Schiavon B, Viola TW, Grassi-Oliveira R. Theoretical models about single or multiple processes of executive functions. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2012;4:29-34. In addition, EFs impact affective-emotional, motivational, and social skills.33. Kluwe-Schiavon B, Viola TW, Grassi-Oliveira R. Theoretical models about single or multiple processes of executive functions. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2012;4:29-34. , 44. Uehara E, Charchat-Fichman H, Landeira-Fernandez J. Executive functions? An integrative portrait of the main models and theories of this concept. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:25-37. EFs are especially important in older adults for performing and troubleshooting routine tasks, from the most simple to the most complex. Executive functions comprise the six domains described below. Planning refers to the identification of a sequence of actions required to achieve a goal. Efficient planning includes thinking about alternatives and choosing the most effective one.44. Uehara E, Charchat-Fichman H, Landeira-Fernandez J. Executive functions? An integrative portrait of the main models and theories of this concept. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:25-37. Working memory is defined as a system of temporary storage and manipulation of information. This system is activated during the process of learning, language comprehension, reasoning, and production of one's conscience.44. Uehara E, Charchat-Fichman H, Landeira-Fernandez J. Executive functions? An integrative portrait of the main models and theories of this concept. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:25-37. Mental flexibility refers to the ability of alternating between mental sets or tasks, and changing strategies within the same task.55. Lehto JE, Juujaarvi P, Kooistra L, Pulkkinen L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Brit J Develop Psychol 2003;21: 59-80. Inhibitory control refers to the inhibition of a prepotent response, which facilitates the choice of an adequate response and avoids errors.44. Uehara E, Charchat-Fichman H, Landeira-Fernandez J. Executive functions? An integrative portrait of the main models and theories of this concept. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:25-37. Verbal fluency is the ability to generate an appropriate strategy for word searching.66. Brocki KC, Bohlin G. Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Develop Neuropsychol 2004;26: 571-593. Processing speed refers to the time required to process a specific item of information.66. Brocki KC, Bohlin G. Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Develop Neuropsychol 2004;26: 571-593. Neural substrates of these domains are mainly located in the prefrontal cortex of the brain, and are connected to several other brain areas and the central nervous system as a whole.33. Kluwe-Schiavon B, Viola TW, Grassi-Oliveira R. Theoretical models about single or multiple processes of executive functions. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2012;4:29-34.

Executive dysfunction leads to significant limitations in the daily routines of elderly, such as impact on functionality, i.e., in the performance of activities of daily living (ADLs), which reduces autonomy and quality of life.77. Mograbi DC, Faria CA, Charchat-Fichman H, Paradela EMP, Lourenço RA. Relationship between activities of daily living and cognitive ability in a sample of older adults with heterogeneous educational level. Ann Indian Acad Neurol 2014;17:71-76. , 88. Amieva H, Phillips L, Sala SD. Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain Cogn 2003;53:129-132. Deficits in EF found in older patients include inflexibility of thought, reduced working memory processing, difficulty in solving problems, decreased behavioral initiation, perseveration errors, inadequate planning strategies, and disinhibition.11. Espinosa A, Alegret M, Boada M, Vinyes G, et al. Ecological assessment of executive functions in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:751-757. , 88. Amieva H, Phillips L, Sala SD. Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain Cogn 2003;53:129-132. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is considered an intermediate state of cognitive functioning between the changes seen in healthy aging and those typically found in dementia. MCI criteria include self- or informant-reported cognitive complaints, objective cognitive impairment, preserved Independence in functional abilities and no dementia.99. Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J Intern Med 2014; 275:214-228. , 1010. Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman D, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. Arch Neurol 2009;66:1447-1455. Executive dysfunction in aging may appear in MCI or may be an early sign of dementia.11. Espinosa A, Alegret M, Boada M, Vinyes G, et al. Ecological assessment of executive functions in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:751-757.

Executive dysfunction in aging can be measured objectively with neuropsychological tests.1111. Mitchell MB, Shaughnessy LW, Shirk SD, Yang FM, Atri A. Neuropsychological Test Performance and Cognitive Reserve in Healthy Aging and the Alzheimer's Disease Spectrum: A Theoretically-Driven Factor Analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2012;18:1071-1080. There are several executive functions tests available, which vary according to the domains assessed. In general, a neuropsychological test predominantly assesses one of the EF domains. The wide variability of tests, the need for effective measures for early diagnosis of dementia, and the impact of executive dysfunction on ADLs, autonomy, and quality of life of older adults justify the need to identify which instruments are most used to assess executive functions in aging. This aim of this study was to present a systematic review of the instruments most frequently used to assess executive functions in older adults with different educational levels, in clinical and experimental research.

METHODS

A systematic review was conducted on July 30th, 2014 using the PubMed database and combining the search terms "neuropsychological tests", "executive functions", and "mild cognitive impairment", with no language restriction. The study included articles that fulfilled the following criteria: [1] all participants aged over 50 years; [2] healthy older adults, MCI or dementia; [3] neuropsychological tests used to assess executive functions; [4] published in the last 5 years; and [5] cross-sectional or longitudinal studies. The studies that fulfilled the following criteria were excluded: [1] review studies; [2] studies with psychiatric or neurological patients, except for dementia and MCI. To assess which executive functions tests were most commonly used, a percentage of usage of over 20% was adopted as the counting criterion, considering only the articles that met the inclusion and exclusion criteria.

RESULTS

Seventy-four articles published between July 2009 and July 2014 were found in Pubmed. Of these, only 25 fulfilled all the inclusion criteria and 49 articles were excluded according to the exclusion criteria.

Seven tests of executive function were used in more than 20% of the selected articles: [1] Trail Making Test (TMT) Form B; [2] Verbal Fluency Test (VFT) - F, A and S; [3] VFT Animals category; [4] Clock Drawing Test (CDT); [5] Digits Forward and Backward subtests (WAIS-R or WAIS-III); [6] Stroop Test; and [7] Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and its variations. The EF domains most frequently studied by the researchers were mental flexibility, verbal fluency, planning, working memory, inhibitory control, and processing speed. Table 1 describes the relationship between the most used neuropsychological tests and their cognitive domains.

Table 1.
The seven executive function tests most frequently used in aging research.

Table 2 presents all the executive function tests used in the 25 articles, with their respective frequencies and percentage of usage. Table 2 also shows the number of studies by domain evaluated.

Table 2.
Frequency of use of executive function tests in aging research.

Few studies chose to use only one EF test, considering only one domain, and most studies combined two or more EF domains, as follows: [1] Only one EF domain: 4 studies; of these, three were interested in the psychometric properties or validation of a specific test and one study opted for a mental flexibility test as the single measure of executive function; [2] Combining two EF domains: five studies; of these, four chose one or more verbal fluency tests plus one inhibitory control test, planning test or mental flexibility test; only one study combined tests assessing mental flexibility and inhibitory control; [3] Combining three EF domains: nine studies; [4] Combining four EF domains: six studies; [5] Combining five EF domains: only one study; all domains except for inhibitory control were included in this study. These results, together with the references of the studies, are shown in Table 3.

Table 3.
Frequency of studies by number of domains used to evaluate executive functions in aging research.

DISCUSSION

The systematic review presented here identified that the tests most frequently used to assess executive functions in aging research in the last five years were: [1] Trail Making Test Form B, [2] Verbal Fluency Test - F, A and S, [3] VFT Animals category, [4] Clock Drawing Test, [5] Digits subtests (WAIS-R or WAIS-III), [6] Stroop Test, and [7] Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and its variants (Table 1). The EF domains predominantly evaluated were: [1] mental flexibility (15 studies), [2] verbal fluency (13 studies), [3] planning (13 studies), [4] working memory (13 studies), and [5] inhibitory control (13 studies), as shown in Table 2. Processing speed was evaluated in 6 of the 25 studies (Table 2). The assessment of processing speed is of utmost importance in aging because it is one of the first processes to decline in the early stages of dementia.1212. Thorvaldsson V, Macdonald SW, Fratiglioni L, et al. Onset and rate of cognitive change before dementia diagnosis: findings from two Swedish population-based longitudinal studies. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2011; 17:154-162. However, processing speed was assessed in only six studies. Most of the selected articles favored the use of tests involving the most complex processes of executive functions that require greater action control.

As seen in Table 3, none of the 25 studies selected evaluated all six domains and only one study combined EF tests covering five domains.3333. Cercy SP. The Verbal Clock Test: preliminary validation of a brief, vision- and motor-free measure of executive function in a clinical sample. Clin Neuropsychol 2012;26:1312-1341. This can be explained by the attempt to reduce the time spent on a long neuropsychological assessment. In other words, if many neuropsychological tests were used to evaluate EF domains, in addition to tests assessing other cognitive functions, data collection would take too long, resulting in high dropout rates and making it difficult to conduct research with a large sample.4141. Bangen KJ, Jak AJ, Schiehser DM, et al. Complex activities of daily living vary by mild cognitive impairment subtype. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:630-639. , 4646. Vicini-Chilovi B, Riva M, Conti M, et al. Does age at observation time affect the clinical presentation of mild cognitive impairment? Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;30:212-218. However, important EF data is lost when some domains are not covered in a neuropsychological assessment.

There is no established criterion in the literature with respect to the optimal number of tests needed to evaluate executive functions, but, according to the results of the present review, combining 3 to 4 EF domains in comprehensive batteries was the most frequent procedure adopted.2121. Leyhe T, Saur R, Eschweiler GW, Milian M. Clock test deficits are associated with semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2009;22:235-245. , 2222. de Paula JJ, Bertola L, Ávila RT, et al. Clinical applicability and cutoff values for an unstructured neuropsychological assessment protocol for older adults with low formal education. PLoS One 2013;8:e73167. , 2929. Jacus JP, Gély-Nargeot MC. Impact of dysexecutive syndrome on autonomy in early Alzheimer's disease and Mild cognitive impairment. Geriatr Psychol Neuropsychiatr Vieil 2014;12:101-112. , 3131. Bastug G, Ozel-Kizil ET, Sakarya A, Altintas O, Kirici S, Altunoz U. Oral trail making task as a discriminative tool for different levels of cognitive impairment and normal aging. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2013;28:411-417. , 3535. Summers MJ, Saunders NL. Neuropsychological measures predict decline to Alzheimer's dementia from mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 2012;26:498-508.

36. Malek-Ahmadi M, Raj A, Small BJ. Semantic clustering as a neuropsychological predictor for amnestic-MCI. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2011;18:280-292.

37. Chang YL, Bondi MW, McEvoy LK, et al. Global clinical dementia rating of 0.5 in MCI masks variability related to level of function. Neurology 2011;76:652-659.

38. Yubero R, Gil P, Paul N, Maestú F. Influence of memory strategies on memory test performance: a study in healthy and pathological aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2011;18:497-515.

39. Nordlund A, Rolstad S, Göthlin M, Edman A, Hansen S, Wallin A. Cognitive profiles of incipient dementia in the Goteborg MCI study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;30:403-410.
- 4040. Pereira FS, Yassuda MS, Oliveira AM, et al. Profiles of functional deficits in mild cognitive impairment and dementia: benefits from objective measurement. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:297-305. , 4242. Karantzoulis S, Troyer AK, Rich JB. Prospective memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:407-415. , 4343. Paula JJ, Miranda DM, Moraes EN, Malloy-Diniz LF. Mapping the clockworks: what does the Clock Drawing Test assess in normal and pathological aging? Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2013;71:763-768. , 4545. Bombin I, Santiago-Ramajo S, Garolera M, et al. Functional impairment as a defining feature of: amnestic MCI cognitive, emotional, and demographic correlates. Int Psychogeriatr 2012;24:1494-1504. , 4949. Rainville C, Lepage E, Gauthier S, Kergoat MJ, Belleville S. Executive function deficits in persons with mild cognitive impairment: a study with a Tower of London task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2012;34:306-324. The choice of three or more combinations selected by the majority of studies in this review2121. Leyhe T, Saur R, Eschweiler GW, Milian M. Clock test deficits are associated with semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2009;22:235-245. , 2222. de Paula JJ, Bertola L, Ávila RT, et al. Clinical applicability and cutoff values for an unstructured neuropsychological assessment protocol for older adults with low formal education. PLoS One 2013;8:e73167. , 2929. Jacus JP, Gély-Nargeot MC. Impact of dysexecutive syndrome on autonomy in early Alzheimer's disease and Mild cognitive impairment. Geriatr Psychol Neuropsychiatr Vieil 2014;12:101-112.

30. Gaudreau G, Monetta L, Macoir J, Laforce R Jr, Poulin S, Hudon C. Verbal irony comprehension in older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 2013;27:702-712.
- 3131. Bastug G, Ozel-Kizil ET, Sakarya A, Altintas O, Kirici S, Altunoz U. Oral trail making task as a discriminative tool for different levels of cognitive impairment and normal aging. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2013;28:411-417. , 3333. Cercy SP. The Verbal Clock Test: preliminary validation of a brief, vision- and motor-free measure of executive function in a clinical sample. Clin Neuropsychol 2012;26:1312-1341. , 3535. Summers MJ, Saunders NL. Neuropsychological measures predict decline to Alzheimer's dementia from mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 2012;26:498-508.

36. Malek-Ahmadi M, Raj A, Small BJ. Semantic clustering as a neuropsychological predictor for amnestic-MCI. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2011;18:280-292.

37. Chang YL, Bondi MW, McEvoy LK, et al. Global clinical dementia rating of 0.5 in MCI masks variability related to level of function. Neurology 2011;76:652-659.

38. Yubero R, Gil P, Paul N, Maestú F. Influence of memory strategies on memory test performance: a study in healthy and pathological aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2011;18:497-515.

39. Nordlund A, Rolstad S, Göthlin M, Edman A, Hansen S, Wallin A. Cognitive profiles of incipient dementia in the Goteborg MCI study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;30:403-410.
- 4040. Pereira FS, Yassuda MS, Oliveira AM, et al. Profiles of functional deficits in mild cognitive impairment and dementia: benefits from objective measurement. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:297-305. , 4242. Karantzoulis S, Troyer AK, Rich JB. Prospective memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:407-415. , 4343. Paula JJ, Miranda DM, Moraes EN, Malloy-Diniz LF. Mapping the clockworks: what does the Clock Drawing Test assess in normal and pathological aging? Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2013;71:763-768. , 4545. Bombin I, Santiago-Ramajo S, Garolera M, et al. Functional impairment as a defining feature of: amnestic MCI cognitive, emotional, and demographic correlates. Int Psychogeriatr 2012;24:1494-1504. , 4949. Rainville C, Lepage E, Gauthier S, Kergoat MJ, Belleville S. Executive function deficits in persons with mild cognitive impairment: a study with a Tower of London task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2012;34:306-324. (Table 3) is because EFs are a multidimensional construct, i.e. comprise a series of interrelated skills and high-level cognitive processing and recruit several domains in parallel, such as mental flexibility, planning, verbal fluency, inhibitory control, processing speed, and working memory.33. Kluwe-Schiavon B, Viola TW, Grassi-Oliveira R. Theoretical models about single or multiple processes of executive functions. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2012;4:29-34. , 44. Uehara E, Charchat-Fichman H, Landeira-Fernandez J. Executive functions? An integrative portrait of the main models and theories of this concept. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:25-37. Researchers seem to avoid using only one or two EF measures in order to ensure that most of the EF domains are covered. A few studies2323. Forti P, Olivelli V, Rietti E, Maltoni B, Ravaglia G. Diagnostic performance of an Executive Clock Drawing Task (CLOX) as a screening test for mild cognitive impairment in elderly persons with cognitive complaints. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;30:20-27. , 3232. Parra MA, Ascencio LL, Urquina HF, Manes F, Ibáñez AM. P300 and neuropsychological assessment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer dementia. Front Neurol 2012;3:172. , 4141. Bangen KJ, Jak AJ, Schiehser DM, et al. Complex activities of daily living vary by mild cognitive impairment subtype. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:630-639. , 4444. Schroeter ML, Vogt B, Frisch S, et al. Executive deficits are related to the inferior frontal junction in early dementia. Brain 2012;135:201-215. , 4646. Vicini-Chilovi B, Riva M, Conti M, et al. Does age at observation time affect the clinical presentation of mild cognitive impairment? Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;30:212-218. (Table 3) restricted the EF assessment to two tests, usually combining a flexibility, planning or inhibitory control test with a verbal fluency test, which is quick, easy-to-apply and sensitive for discriminating people with dementia, MCI and healthy older adults.1313. Caramelli P, Carthery MT, Porto SP, Charchat-Fichman H, Nitrini, R. Category fluency as a screening test for Alzheimer disease in illiterate and literate patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2007;21:65-67.

As seen in Table 2, the TMT Form B and WCST and its variants were the most frequently selected tests for assessing mental flexibility. The TMT Form B was used in nine studies (from a total of 14 studies assessing flexibility) whereas the WCST or one of its variants were used in six studies. The TMT Form B is predominantly a mental flexibility measure.1414. Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data strati?ed by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2004;19:203-214. , 5050. Oliveira-Souza R, Moll J, Passman LJ, et al. Trail making and cognitive set-shifting. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2000;58:826-829. The individual must switch their focus of attention repeatedly between two sequences (numerical and alphabetical). Mental engagement, motor dexterity, and working memory are also recruited during the test.1414. Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data strati?ed by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2004;19:203-214. , 5050. Oliveira-Souza R, Moll J, Passman LJ, et al. Trail making and cognitive set-shifting. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2000;58:826-829. The WCST and its variants also assess mental flexibility.5151. Fernandes-Lopes R & Lima-Agrimon, I. Idosos com diabetes mellitus tipo 2 e o desempenho cognitivo no teste Wisconsin de classificação de cartas (WCST). Universitas Psychol 2010;9:697-713. The subject must migrate from one classification rule to another during the test. Selective attention and impulsivity are also evaluated in this test.5151. Fernandes-Lopes R & Lima-Agrimon, I. Idosos com diabetes mellitus tipo 2 e o desempenho cognitivo no teste Wisconsin de classificação de cartas (WCST). Universitas Psychol 2010;9:697-713. The fact that the TMT Form B has a simpler and faster application procedure than the WCST and its variants justifies its selection by most studies.

Other tests commonly used to evaluate executive functions in aging were the VFT - F, A and S and the VFT Animals category. These tests were mainly chosen for the evaluation of verbal fluency. Seven studies that assessed this domain chose the VFT - F, A and while six chose the VFT Animals category. The VFT - F, A and S evaluate verbal fluency and entail an active search for specific information in memory.1717. Machado TH, Charchat-Fichman H, Santos EL, et al. Normative data for healthy elderly on the phonemic verbal fluency task - FAS. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:55-60. The VFT Animals category predominantly evaluates verbal fluency.1919. Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Nitrini R, et al. Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:49-54. During execution of the task, there is an active search for information falling into a certain category. Language and semantic memory are also evaluated in this test.1919. Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Nitrini R, et al. Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:49-54.

Another frequently used task was the CDT, which was chosen by six studies out of a total of 13 that evaluated planning. Planning is the main cognitive process recruited during the CDT, since the circle should be large enough to accommodate the 12 numbers of the clock, which in turn should be equally distributed and correctly positioned. In addition, the hands must mark the requested time, with the minute hand greater than the hour hand. Visual constructive skills are also evaluated in this test.1818. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bottino CMC, Damasceno BP, Brucki SMD, Anghinah R. Diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in Brazil: cognitive and functional evaluation. Recommendations of the Scientifc Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2005;63:720-727. CDT is simple and widely used in cognitive aging research and for cognitive impairment screening in geriatric services.1818. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bottino CMC, Damasceno BP, Brucki SMD, Anghinah R. Diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in Brazil: cognitive and functional evaluation. Recommendations of the Scientifc Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2005;63:720-727.

A working memory measure, the Digits subtest (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) was the most frequently used, having been chosen by six out of thirteen studies. This test encompasses two tasks, forward and the backward. A detailed description of the test can be found in Table 1. The Digits forward subtest (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) evaluates information storage by verbal working memory. A string of digits is dictated by the examiner and the participant has to repeat it immediately after. The Digits backward subtest (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) measures verbal content processing in working memory.

Finally, the Stroop test was the most frequently chosen test (6 out of 11 studies) for evaluating inhibitory control. It assesses the subject's ability to inhibit an automatic behavior (reading a word) and perform a controlled behavior (saying the color the word is printed in).

Executive functions undergo changes during the normal aging process, that is, performance on EF tests is expected to deteriorate with increasing age.1414. Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data strati?ed by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2004;19:203-214. , 1515. Hamdan AC, Hamdan EMLR. Effects of age and education level on the Trail Making Test in A healthy Brazilian sample. Psychol Neurosci 2009;2:199-203. , 1717. Machado TH, Charchat-Fichman H, Santos EL, et al. Normative data for healthy elderly on the phonemic verbal fluency task - FAS. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:55-60.

18. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bottino CMC, Damasceno BP, Brucki SMD, Anghinah R. Diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in Brazil: cognitive and functional evaluation. Recommendations of the Scientifc Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2005;63:720-727.

19. Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Nitrini R, et al. Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:49-54.
- 2020. Agrell B, Dehljn O. The clock-drawing test. Age Ageing 1998;27:399-403. , 2525. Choi HJ, Lee DY, Seo EH, et al. A Normative Study of the Digit Span in an Educationally Diverse Elderly Population. Psychiatry Investig 2014; 11:39-43.

26. Bayard S, Erkes J, Moroni C. Victoria Stroop Test: Normative Data in a Sample Group of Older People and the Study of Their Clinical Applications in the Assessment of Inhibition in Alzheimer's Disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2011;26:653-661.
- 2727. Klein M, Adda CC, Miotto EC, Lucia MCS, Scaff M. O Paradigma Stroop Em Uma Amostra De Idosos Brasileiros. Psicol Hosp 2010;8:93-112. Decline in EF during the aging process can be mild or severe. Older people with a significant decline in executive functions fall within the Cognitive Impairment Mild (MCI) diagnostic category and may progress to dementia.5252. Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Oliveira RM, Caramelli P, Aguiar D, Novaes R. Disexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment predominance in elderly assisted at geriatric clinic into public hospital in Rio de Janeiro. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:31-40. Executive dysfunction in aging is related to a reduced ability to perform activities of daily living, which creates dependency, loss of autonomy, and reduced quality of life,77. Mograbi DC, Faria CA, Charchat-Fichman H, Paradela EMP, Lourenço RA. Relationship between activities of daily living and cognitive ability in a sample of older adults with heterogeneous educational level. Ann Indian Acad Neurol 2014;17:71-76. justifying the importance of investigating the functioning of EF in the elderly population and the main tests discussed in the present review.

Besides the age effect, EF tests are affected by education, that is, the lower the subject's educational level, the worse the test performance.1414. Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data strati?ed by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2004;19:203-214. , 1515. Hamdan AC, Hamdan EMLR. Effects of age and education level on the Trail Making Test in A healthy Brazilian sample. Psychol Neurosci 2009;2:199-203. , 1717. Machado TH, Charchat-Fichman H, Santos EL, et al. Normative data for healthy elderly on the phonemic verbal fluency task - FAS. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:55-60.

18. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bottino CMC, Damasceno BP, Brucki SMD, Anghinah R. Diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in Brazil: cognitive and functional evaluation. Recommendations of the Scientifc Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2005;63:720-727.

19. Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Nitrini R, et al. Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:49-54.
- 2020. Agrell B, Dehljn O. The clock-drawing test. Age Ageing 1998;27:399-403. , 2525. Choi HJ, Lee DY, Seo EH, et al. A Normative Study of the Digit Span in an Educationally Diverse Elderly Population. Psychiatry Investig 2014; 11:39-43.

26. Bayard S, Erkes J, Moroni C. Victoria Stroop Test: Normative Data in a Sample Group of Older People and the Study of Their Clinical Applications in the Assessment of Inhibition in Alzheimer's Disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2011;26:653-661.

27. Klein M, Adda CC, Miotto EC, Lucia MCS, Scaff M. O Paradigma Stroop Em Uma Amostra De Idosos Brasileiros. Psicol Hosp 2010;8:93-112.
- 2828. Abe M, Suzuki K, Okada K, et al. Normative data on tests for frontal lobe functions: Trail Making Test, Verbal fluency, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Keio version). No To Shinkei 2004;56:567-574. The Brazilian older population has a higher educational variability than older people in developed countries.5353. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Porto CS, et al. Brief cognitive battery in the diagnosis of mild Alzheimer's disease in subjects with medium and high levels of education. Dement Neuropsychol 2007;1:32-36. The relationship between education and cognition is widely acknowledged in the literature. Foss et al.5454. Foss MP, Carvalho VA, Machado TH, et al. Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) Normative data for the Brazilian middle-age and elderly populations. Dement Neuropsychol 2013;7:374-379. conducted a study showing that educational level is strongly correlated with subtests of executive functions and other cognitive functions (attention, visual constructive skills, language, memory, and global cognition score). The sample consisted of 502 Brazilian older adults without cognitive deficits. The results indicated that illiterate older adults performed significantly worse than participants with 1 year or more of formal education. Participants with 1 or 2 years of education had a significantly worse performance on all subtests than participants with 3 or 4 years of education which, in turn, performed worse than the group with 11 or more years of formal education. Groups with 5-10 years of education showed no differences in performance.

There is evidence in the literature that the performance of executive functions in older adults is also influenced by socioeconomic status.5252. Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Oliveira RM, Caramelli P, Aguiar D, Novaes R. Disexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment predominance in elderly assisted at geriatric clinic into public hospital in Rio de Janeiro. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:31-40. Charchat-Fichman et al.5252. Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Oliveira RM, Caramelli P, Aguiar D, Novaes R. Disexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment predominance in elderly assisted at geriatric clinic into public hospital in Rio de Janeiro. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:31-40. mapped the neuropsychological profile of a sample of 88 elderly people without dementia at a geriatric outpatient clinic of a public hospital in the city of Rio de Janeiro. Patients with traumatic brain injury, stroke, Parkinson's disease, and other neurological or psychiatric disorders previously diagnosed by the medical team, were excluded. Charchat-Fichman et al.5252. Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Oliveira RM, Caramelli P, Aguiar D, Novaes R. Disexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment predominance in elderly assisted at geriatric clinic into public hospital in Rio de Janeiro. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:31-40. concluded that 94.3% of the sample had significant EF impairment. The researchers hypothesized that this result was a reflection of the low socioeconomic status of the sample, noting that perhaps the level of education is not the only factor involved in EF decline among elderly. Socioeconomic variables may contribute to the low performance of elderly on EF tests.5252. Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Oliveira RM, Caramelli P, Aguiar D, Novaes R. Disexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment predominance in elderly assisted at geriatric clinic into public hospital in Rio de Janeiro. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:31-40. These data raises the question of whether the tests commonly used to assess EF in aging are suitable for the Brazilian population.

This study identified the tests and specific domains most frequently used in the last five years to assess EF in older adults. To our knowledge, there are no other review studies in the literature on EF testing during aging. This study raises the issue of the adequacy of the most frequently used tests for use in the Brazilian older population, considering the inherent variability in educational and socio-demographic levels, which result in heterogeneity of cognitive test outcomes. Knowing which EF tests and domains are chosen by the main research groups can direct future research and help build appropriate EF assessment protocols for different educational levels and socio-demographic profiles of older adults in Brazil.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Espinosa A, Alegret M, Boada M, Vinyes G, et al. Ecological assessment of executive functions in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:751-757.
  • 2
    Diamond A. Executive Functions. Annu Rev Psychol 2013;64:135-68.
  • 3
    Kluwe-Schiavon B, Viola TW, Grassi-Oliveira R. Theoretical models about single or multiple processes of executive functions. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2012;4:29-34.
  • 4
    Uehara E, Charchat-Fichman H, Landeira-Fernandez J. Executive functions? An integrative portrait of the main models and theories of this concept. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:25-37.
  • 5
    Lehto JE, Juujaarvi P, Kooistra L, Pulkkinen L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Brit J Develop Psychol 2003;21: 59-80.
  • 6
    Brocki KC, Bohlin G. Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Develop Neuropsychol 2004;26: 571-593.
  • 7
    Mograbi DC, Faria CA, Charchat-Fichman H, Paradela EMP, Lourenço RA. Relationship between activities of daily living and cognitive ability in a sample of older adults with heterogeneous educational level. Ann Indian Acad Neurol 2014;17:71-76.
  • 8
    Amieva H, Phillips L, Sala SD. Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain Cogn 2003;53:129-132.
  • 9
    Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J Intern Med 2014; 275:214-228.
  • 10
    Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman D, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. Arch Neurol 2009;66:1447-1455.
  • 11
    Mitchell MB, Shaughnessy LW, Shirk SD, Yang FM, Atri A. Neuropsychological Test Performance and Cognitive Reserve in Healthy Aging and the Alzheimer's Disease Spectrum: A Theoretically-Driven Factor Analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2012;18:1071-1080.
  • 12
    Thorvaldsson V, Macdonald SW, Fratiglioni L, et al. Onset and rate of cognitive change before dementia diagnosis: findings from two Swedish population-based longitudinal studies. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2011; 17:154-162.
  • 13
    Caramelli P, Carthery MT, Porto SP, Charchat-Fichman H, Nitrini, R. Category fluency as a screening test for Alzheimer disease in illiterate and literate patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2007;21:65-67.
  • 14
    Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data strati?ed by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2004;19:203-214.
  • 15
    Hamdan AC, Hamdan EMLR. Effects of age and education level on the Trail Making Test in A healthy Brazilian sample. Psychol Neurosci 2009;2:199-203.
  • 16
    Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative Data Stratified by Age and Education for Two Measures of Verbal Fluency: FAS and Animal Naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1999;14:167-177.
  • 17
    Machado TH, Charchat-Fichman H, Santos EL, et al. Normative data for healthy elderly on the phonemic verbal fluency task - FAS. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:55-60.
  • 18
    Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bottino CMC, Damasceno BP, Brucki SMD, Anghinah R. Diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in Brazil: cognitive and functional evaluation. Recommendations of the Scientifc Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2005;63:720-727.
  • 19
    Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Nitrini R, et al. Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:49-54.
  • 20
    Agrell B, Dehljn O. The clock-drawing test. Age Ageing 1998;27:399-403.
  • 21
    Leyhe T, Saur R, Eschweiler GW, Milian M. Clock test deficits are associated with semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2009;22:235-245.
  • 22
    de Paula JJ, Bertola L, Ávila RT, et al. Clinical applicability and cutoff values for an unstructured neuropsychological assessment protocol for older adults with low formal education. PLoS One 2013;8:e73167.
  • 23
    Forti P, Olivelli V, Rietti E, Maltoni B, Ravaglia G. Diagnostic performance of an Executive Clock Drawing Task (CLOX) as a screening test for mild cognitive impairment in elderly persons with cognitive complaints. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;30:20-27.
  • 24
    Fabricio AT, Aprahamian I, Yassuda MS. Qualitative analysis of the Clock Drawing Test by educational level and cognitive profile. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2014;72(4):289-295.
  • 25
    Choi HJ, Lee DY, Seo EH, et al. A Normative Study of the Digit Span in an Educationally Diverse Elderly Population. Psychiatry Investig 2014; 11:39-43.
  • 26
    Bayard S, Erkes J, Moroni C. Victoria Stroop Test: Normative Data in a Sample Group of Older People and the Study of Their Clinical Applications in the Assessment of Inhibition in Alzheimer's Disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2011;26:653-661.
  • 27
    Klein M, Adda CC, Miotto EC, Lucia MCS, Scaff M. O Paradigma Stroop Em Uma Amostra De Idosos Brasileiros. Psicol Hosp 2010;8:93-112.
  • 28
    Abe M, Suzuki K, Okada K, et al. Normative data on tests for frontal lobe functions: Trail Making Test, Verbal fluency, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Keio version). No To Shinkei 2004;56:567-574.
  • 29
    Jacus JP, Gély-Nargeot MC. Impact of dysexecutive syndrome on autonomy in early Alzheimer's disease and Mild cognitive impairment. Geriatr Psychol Neuropsychiatr Vieil 2014;12:101-112.
  • 30
    Gaudreau G, Monetta L, Macoir J, Laforce R Jr, Poulin S, Hudon C. Verbal irony comprehension in older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 2013;27:702-712.
  • 31
    Bastug G, Ozel-Kizil ET, Sakarya A, Altintas O, Kirici S, Altunoz U. Oral trail making task as a discriminative tool for different levels of cognitive impairment and normal aging. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2013;28:411-417.
  • 32
    Parra MA, Ascencio LL, Urquina HF, Manes F, Ibáñez AM. P300 and neuropsychological assessment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer dementia. Front Neurol 2012;3:172.
  • 33
    Cercy SP. The Verbal Clock Test: preliminary validation of a brief, vision- and motor-free measure of executive function in a clinical sample. Clin Neuropsychol 2012;26:1312-1341.
  • 34
    Berna F, Schönknecht P, Seidl U, Toro P, Schröder J. Episodic autobiographical memory in normal aging and mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. Psychiatry Res 2012;200:807-812.
  • 35
    Summers MJ, Saunders NL. Neuropsychological measures predict decline to Alzheimer's dementia from mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 2012;26:498-508.
  • 36
    Malek-Ahmadi M, Raj A, Small BJ. Semantic clustering as a neuropsychological predictor for amnestic-MCI. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2011;18:280-292.
  • 37
    Chang YL, Bondi MW, McEvoy LK, et al. Global clinical dementia rating of 0.5 in MCI masks variability related to level of function. Neurology 2011;76:652-659.
  • 38
    Yubero R, Gil P, Paul N, Maestú F. Influence of memory strategies on memory test performance: a study in healthy and pathological aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2011;18:497-515.
  • 39
    Nordlund A, Rolstad S, Göthlin M, Edman A, Hansen S, Wallin A. Cognitive profiles of incipient dementia in the Goteborg MCI study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;30:403-410.
  • 40
    Pereira FS, Yassuda MS, Oliveira AM, et al. Profiles of functional deficits in mild cognitive impairment and dementia: benefits from objective measurement. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:297-305.
  • 41
    Bangen KJ, Jak AJ, Schiehser DM, et al. Complex activities of daily living vary by mild cognitive impairment subtype. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:630-639.
  • 42
    Karantzoulis S, Troyer AK, Rich JB. Prospective memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:407-415.
  • 43
    Paula JJ, Miranda DM, Moraes EN, Malloy-Diniz LF. Mapping the clockworks: what does the Clock Drawing Test assess in normal and pathological aging? Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2013;71:763-768.
  • 44
    Schroeter ML, Vogt B, Frisch S, et al. Executive deficits are related to the inferior frontal junction in early dementia. Brain 2012;135:201-215.
  • 45
    Bombin I, Santiago-Ramajo S, Garolera M, et al. Functional impairment as a defining feature of: amnestic MCI cognitive, emotional, and demographic correlates. Int Psychogeriatr 2012;24:1494-1504.
  • 46
    Vicini-Chilovi B, Riva M, Conti M, et al. Does age at observation time affect the clinical presentation of mild cognitive impairment? Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;30:212-218.
  • 47
    Costa Armentano CG, Porto CS, Nitrini R, Dozzi Brucki SM. Ecological evaluation of executive functions in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2013;27:95-101.
  • 48
    de Paula JJ, Moreira L, Nicolato R, de Marco LA. The Tower of London Test: different scoring criteria for diagnosing Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Psychol Rep 2012;110:477-488.
  • 49
    Rainville C, Lepage E, Gauthier S, Kergoat MJ, Belleville S. Executive function deficits in persons with mild cognitive impairment: a study with a Tower of London task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2012;34:306-324.
  • 50
    Oliveira-Souza R, Moll J, Passman LJ, et al. Trail making and cognitive set-shifting. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2000;58:826-829.
  • 51
    Fernandes-Lopes R & Lima-Agrimon, I. Idosos com diabetes mellitus tipo 2 e o desempenho cognitivo no teste Wisconsin de classificação de cartas (WCST). Universitas Psychol 2010;9:697-713.
  • 52
    Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Oliveira RM, Caramelli P, Aguiar D, Novaes R. Disexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment predominance in elderly assisted at geriatric clinic into public hospital in Rio de Janeiro. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam 2013;5:31-40.
  • 53
    Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Porto CS, et al. Brief cognitive battery in the diagnosis of mild Alzheimer's disease in subjects with medium and high levels of education. Dement Neuropsychol 2007;1:32-36.
  • 54
    Foss MP, Carvalho VA, Machado TH, et al. Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) Normative data for the Brazilian middle-age and elderly populations. Dement Neuropsychol 2013;7:374-379.
  • This study was conducted at the Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Apr-Jun 2015

History

  • Received
    10 Feb 2015
  • Accepted
    18 Apr 2015
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br