Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Study on the training of future teachers in reading comprehension and written expression strategies * * English versión by Ayako Koike.

Abstract

For the construction of knowledge in the university environment, reading comprehension and written expression are essential competences, and also constitute the necessary base to carry out activities such as processing information. As many students of universities of Education will be teachers, we wonder if these students will really acquire adequate skills in reading comprehension and written expression to successfully carry out their professional work. This study is presented in two research designs. The first one takes into account the formative moment as a variable; it analyzes the competences in written comprehension and expression of the students accessing the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education, and also of those who, after its completion, have accessed to the University Degree in Psychopedagogy. Thus, we consider the group of students of the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education as the Pre-test measure; the formative process before becoming students of the University Degree in Psychopedagogy as the treatment or test; and these actual students as the Post-test measure. The second one considers the growing influence of ICTs on learning environments, so it analyzes whether there are remarkable differences according to the stimulus variable presented - printed texts and hypertexts - when applying the abovementioned skills to summarize as an academic task. Despite of the fact that the students of the University Degree in Psychopedagogy have received three years of academic training as future Primary Education teachers, the results showed the same lack of reading comprehension and written expression skills as those at the beginning step of the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education.

Higher education; Teacher training; Reading, writing

Resumen

La comprensión lectora y la expresión escrita son competencias fundamentales para la construcción de conocimiento en el ámbito universitario, ya que constituyen la base necesaria para realizar actividades como la síntesis de información. Dado que muchos estudiantes de las facultades de Educación serán futuros docentes, nos planteamos si realmente dichos estudiantes adquirirán competencias de comprensión lectora y expresión escrita adecuadas para desempeñar con éxito dicha labor profesional. Este estudio, presentado en dos diseños de investigación, analiza en primer lugar, y considerando como variable el momento formativo, las competencias en comprensión y expresión escrita del alumnado que accede al grado de Maestro; y de quienes lo han concluido y estudian posteriormente la licenciatura en Psicopedagogía. Para ello, consideramos al grupo de estudiantes del grado de Maestro como medida Pre-test, consideramos el proceso formativo por el que habían pasado los alumnos de la licenciatura de Psicopedagogía como tratamiento o test, y establecimos a estos últimos alumnos como medida Pos-test. En segundo lugar, y considerando que los entornos de aprendizaje reciben la creciente influencia de las TIC, se analiza si existen posibles diferencias atendiendo a la variable estímulo presentado -textos impresos e hipertextos- al aplicar las habilidades mencionadas en una tarea académica de resumen. Los resultados obtenidos muestran que los alumnos de la Licenciatura de Psicopedagogía, pese a haber recibido durante tres años formación académica en los estudios de Maestro, mostraban las mismas carencias de habilidades de comprensión lectora y expresión escrita que los alumnos que comenzaban el grado de Maestro.

Enseñanza superior; Formación de profesores; Lectura, escritura

Introduction

The starting point of the present article is rooted in the growing concern of lecturers in the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education about the difficulties of their students, i.e. future teachers, to use basic skills in reading comprehension and written expression for the construction of knowledge and psychoeducation. This situation has been already shown in previous studies (HERRADA; QUINTERO, 2012 HERRADA , Gabriel ; QUINTERO , Anunciación . Habilidades básicas implicadas en la construcción del conocimiento en los alumnos universitarios a través de textos hipervinculados . In: JORNADAS UNIVERSITARIAS DE TECNOLOGÍA EDUCATIVA, 19 ., 2011 , Sevilla . Anales… Sevilla : Universidad de Sevilla , 2011 . p. 778 - 786 .; ARROYO; JIMÉNEZ-BAENA, 2016 ARROYO , Rosario ; JIMÉNEZ-BAENA , Abrahan F . Estructuración del ensayo científico sobre contenidos interculturales y competencias escritoras de estudiantes universitarios . Revista de Investigación Educativa , Murcia , v. 34 , n. 2 , p. 351 - 367 , 2016 . ; DUGAREVA; DIEGUEZ; PÉREZ, 2014 DUGAREVA , Elena A. ; DIÉGUEZ , Raquel ; PÉREZ , Nereyda . Modelo didáctico del proceso de formación comunicativa científica escrita de los estudiantes universitarios . Pedagogía Universitaria , La Habana , v. 19 , n. 4 , p. 55 - 68 , 2014 .; RELLO; MERCADER; SIEGENTHALER; PRESENTATION, 2018 RELLO , Javier et al . Funcionamiento ejecutivo y comprensión lectora al finalizar educación primaria . International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology , Badajoz , v. 1 , n. 2 , p. 367 - 376 , 2018 .), as well as in PISA or PIAAC surveys: the Programme for International Student Assessment (MECD, 2015) and Program for the International Assessment of Adult Competencies (MECD, 2013) respectively and show a gloomy picture of the future because of the requirements of the students to excel in competencies to become knowledge builders, especially in the university context.

Considering the abovementioned aspects, this study has a double purpose. Firstly, the analysis on the competencies in reading comprehension and written expression of students accessing to the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education, investigating if those students who have faced previous university and academic learning and tasks, as those who have accessed to the University Degree in Psychopedagogy, show some improved command on the strategies involved in text comprehension and expression. And secondly, the consideration of the increasing influence of Information and Communication Technologies (ICTs) on learning environments and, consequently, the analysis on whether there are remarkable differences according to the stimulus presented when applying the abovementioned strategies in an academic task of knowledge access. This stimulus was the presentation of a printed text and a hyperlinked text, and the task consisted of generating a summary after reading both types of texts.

Based on this double purpose, the research establishes a series of aims resulting in different hypotheses. The objectives are as follows:

  • Describe whether students beginning the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education have the basic skills in reading comprehension and written expression for the knowledge construction.

  • Identify the level of basic skills in reading comprehension and written expression of the students after the completion of a Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education and initiate the University Degree in Psychopedagogy.

  • Compare the level in basic reading comprehension and written expression skills in the two sample groups: Primary Education and Psychopedagogy.

  • Verify the influence of the text types (printed and hyperlinked) on both samples, considering the use of the strategies of reading comprehension and written expression necessary to carry out the summarization.

Theoretical framework

Different authors have highlighted the fact that communicative competencies, including written expression and spelling skills, are not assumed enough by university lecturers ( SANELEUTERIO, 2018 SANELEUTERIO , Elia . El desarrollo de la competencia ortográfica en los estudios de Magisterio . Boletín Redipe , Cali , v. 7 , n. 6 , p. 33 - 38 , 2018 . ). For this reason, the author points out the needed adaptation of the curriculum to improve teacher training in these areas. Being in unison with this author, it is essential to analyze whether these students are skilled enough in reading comprehension and written expression, as these competences are fundamental to train their students in the future. In addition, it is not only to train future lecturers in reading comprehension and written expression techniques, but it is also necessary that they shall be able to evaluate these skills too (RUIZ; VÁZQUEZ; SEVILLANO, 2017 RUIZ , Rocío ; VÁZQUEZ , Esteban ; SEVILLANO , María Luisa . La rúbrica de evaluación de la competencia en expresión escrita: percepción del alumnado sobre su funcionalidad . Ocnos , Cuenca , v. 16 , n. 2 , p. 107 - 110 , 2017 .).

Before approaching the research, some key aspects shall be described; such as the role of microstructure, macrostructure and superstructure in reading comprehension; the -need to use techniques that allow the reader to manage their cognitive resources better during the reading process; and the description and analysis of the synthesis strategies used by mature readers to summarize texts.

Models of text comprehension

Text comprehension is the result of the interaction between the explicit text expressions and the reader knowledge. Some authors have analyzed how mental constructions depend as much on individuals’ representation of textual information as on the reader’s prior knowledge ( TIJERO, 2009 TIJERO , Talia . Representaciones mentales: discusión crítica del modelo de situación de Kintsch , Onomázein , Santiago do Chile , v. 19 , p. 111 - 138 , 2009 . ). Among the best known models we find the one presented by Van Dijk and Kintsch (1983) VAN DIJK , Teun ; KINTSCH , Walter . Strategies of discourse comprehension . New York : Academic Press , 1983 . , and it states that the readers process the text information founded on three levels of representation: surface code, text base, and situation model. Subsequently, Kintsch (1988) KINTSCH , Walter . The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction integration model . Psychological Review , v. 95 , n. 2 , p. 163 - 183 , 1988 . proposed the construction-integration model as the readers construct two types of mental representation during the reading process: the textual representation or base, which is close to the text meaning, and the situational representation, which refers to the context the textual content is framed. In recent years we can highlight models such as the MD-TRACE proposed by ROUET and BRITT (2011) ROUET , Jean-François ; BRITT , M. Anne . Relevance processes in multiple document comprehension . In: MCCRUDDEN , Matthew , MAGLIANO , Joseph ; SCHRAW , Gregory ( Org .) . Relevance instruction and goal-focusing in text learning . Greenwich : Information Age , 2011 . p. 19 - 52 . , which assume that readers use internal resources, such as prior knowledge of the subject matter of the text, the mental representation of the document’s content; external resources, such as external task specifications, the set of sources and documents used; and the products generated by readers, such as summaries or concept maps.

The function of textual microstructure and superstructure in the extraction of the macrostructure

The reader, as s/he reads a text, extracts predicative propositions or outlines formed by a verb and at least two arguments; and s/he also determines the degree of existing coherence between them at a local level. This brings on the elaboration of the microstructure. Thus, it is essential to recognize or infer the two types of relations, thematic and structural, that occur between a proposition and the previous and subsequent proposition ( VAN DIJK, 2006 VAN DIJK , Teun A . Discourse, context and cognition . Discourse Studies , v. 8 , n. 1 , 159 - 177 , 2006 . ).

Apart from identifying propositions and determining the degree of coherence among them, the reader must recognize which of those propositions are textually relevant and establish the coherence among them globally. It requires accessing to a deeper semantic level, called macrostructure, in order to identify and hierarchically reorganize the essential ideas ( ESCORIZA, 2006 ESCORIZA , José . Estrategias de comprensión del discurso escrito expositivo: evaluación e intervención . Barcelona : Universidad de Barcelona , 2006 . ; VAN DIJK, 2006 VAN DIJK , Teun A . Discourse, context and cognition . Discourse Studies , v. 8 , n. 1 , 159 - 177 , 2006 . ). Finally, the textual superstructure or organizational form of the text gains a fundamental role as it provides some keys about which microstructure propositions could form the macrostructure; facilitating, in addition, a scheme, adapted to the different textual typologies (problem-solution, causal, comparative, argumentative, etc.), to store the essential ideas in an organized and hierarchical way.

Cognitive management of the reading process

When the reader reads, does not process the text information at once, but in several parts or processing cycles that retain a reduced number of propositions for a limited time. Taking this as a reference, Sánchez and Solé (2009) SÁNCHEZ , Emilio ; SOLÉ , Isabel . Avanzando en la lectura: acuerdos, contrastes y propuestas , Aula de Innovación Educativa , Barcelona , v. 179 , p. 60 - 64 , 2009 . and Sánchez, García and Rosales (2009, 2010) consider that a text can hardly be processed at a local and global level simultaneously. According to this, McNamara, Ozuru, Best and O’Reilly (2007) state that in order to understand complex texts it is necessary to simplify the task of reading, starting from self-regulating strategies such as re-reading, underlining and taking notes in the text, constructing conceptual maps and summarizing. Different authors have analyzed the use of metacognitive approaches to improve reading comprehension levels (RAMÍREZ; ROSSEL; NAZAR, 2015 RAMÍREZ , Pamela ; ROSSEL , Katherine ; NAZAR , Gabriela . Comprensión lectora y metacognición: análisis de las actividades de lectura en dos textos de estudio de la asignatura de lenguaje y comunicación de séptimo año básico . Estudios Pedagógicos , Valdivia , v. 41 , n. 2 , p. 213 - 231 , 2015 .; CABERO-ALMENARA; PIÑERO; REYES, 2018 CABERO-ALMENARA , Julio ; PIÑERO , Rocío ; REYES , Miguel María . Material educativo multimedia para el aumento de estrategias metacognitivas de comprensión lectora . Perfiles Educativos , Ciudad de México , v. 40 , n. 159 , p. 144 - 159 , 2018 .). Different studies, such as the one presented by Vega, Torres and Del Campo (2017) VEGA , Yuri ; TORRES , Angélica María ; DEL CAMPO , Manuel . Habilidades metamorfológicas y su incidencia en la comprensión lectora . Revista Signos , Valparaíso , v. 50 , n. 95 , p. 453 - 471 , 2017 ., indicate that morphological processing plays a central role in developing reading skills.

Synthesis strategies used by mature and immature readers

There are remarkable differences in the synthesis strategies used by mature and immature readers. Thus, immature readers tend to have comprehension problems both locally and globally, and therefore elaborate a composition closely linked to the surface code of the original text, using omission-selection strategies such as the following: copying (MANDIN; LEMAIRE; DESSUS, 2007 MANDIN , Sonia ; LEMAIRE , Benoît ; DESSUS , Philippe . Modeling summarization assessment strategies with LSA . In: EUROPEAN WORKSHOP ON LATENT SEMANTIC ANALYSIS IN TECHNOLOGY ENHANCED LEARNING, 1 ., 2007 , Heerlen . Anales… Heerlen : Open University of Netherlands , 2007 . p. 20 - 21 .); paraphrasing, almost copying ( HIRVELA; DU, 2013 HIRVELA , Alan ; DU , Qian . Why am I paraphrasing: undergraduate ESL writers’ engagement with source-based academic writing and reading . Journal of English for Academic Purposes , v. 12 , n. 2 , p. 87 - 98 , 2013 . ); and the linked combination, which involves the unstructured integration of two copied or paraphrased sentences or more from the original text. These difficulties in reading comprehension, moreover, often lead this type of reader to construct/infer ideas based on a misinterpretation of the textual content ( MANDIN et al., 2007 MANDIN , Sonia ; LEMAIRE , Benoît ; DESSUS , Philippe . Modeling summarization assessment strategies with LSA . In: EUROPEAN WORKSHOP ON LATENT SEMANTIC ANALYSIS IN TECHNOLOGY ENHANCED LEARNING, 1 ., 2007 , Heerlen . Anales… Heerlen : Open University of Netherlands , 2007 . p. 20 - 21 . ).

Mature readers, on the other hand, have no difficulty in understanding the text locally and globally, so they produce a summary closely linked to the model of the situation, generating a mental model that allows them to integrate the textual base into their previous knowledge. This model is constructed using strategies such as omission-selection, paraphrasing personally, generalization and construction (WESTBY; CULATTA; LAWRENCE; HALL-KENYON, 2010).

The generalization of the use of ICTs has meant that the level of maturity of readers may differ depending on the text format they use. In this sense, some recent studies analyze the reading comprehension using electronic devices. Thus, Martínez and Esquivel (2017) MARTÍNEZ , Waltraud ; ESQUIVEL , Ismael . Efectos de la instrucción de estrategias de lectura, mediadas por TIC, en la comprensión lectora del inglés . Perfiles Educativos , Ciudad de México , v. 39 , n. 157 , p. 105 - 122 , 2017 . analyzed the existing differences in reading comprehension between students using multimedia resources and those reading printed texts: it was found that those students using multimedia resources obtained higher advances than the rest of them. In addition, other authors have demonstrated that multimedia programs facilitate reading comprehension for those students with difficulties (CABERO-ALMENARA; PIÑERO; REYES, 2018 CABERO-ALMENARA , Julio ; PIÑERO , Rocío ; REYES , Miguel María . Material educativo multimedia para el aumento de estrategias metacognitivas de comprensión lectora . Perfiles Educativos , Ciudad de México , v. 40 , n. 159 , p. 144 - 159 , 2018 .).

Methodology

A set of hypotheses is raised based on the objectives described above:

  • New students enrolled in the first year of the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education at the University of Salamanca have difficulties in mastering both basic reading comprehension and written expression skills for the construction of knowledge.

  • Students who access the first year of Psychopedagogy from the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education have difficulties to adequately perform a reading test that requires basic skills in reading comprehension and written expression for the construction of knowledge.

  • There are differences in the levels of basic skills in reading comprehension and written expression between newly enrolled students and those studying Psychopedagogy after finishing the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education.

  • The textual format, printed or hyperlinked, is related to different reading comprehension and written expression tasks.

In order to contrast the hypotheses, two studies are carried out. The first tries to answer the first three hypotheses, while the second focuses on the fourth. Both are framed within different designs that we will describe independently below.

Experimental design

For the first study ( Design I) we established a transversal design with a population with students and graduates of the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education at the University of Salamanca (academic year 2013/2014), in order to select two groups that were at different formative stages:

Primary Education Group ( PED ): students enrolled in the first year of Primary Education Studies at the University of Salamanca.

Psychopedagogy Group ( PSP ): students in the first year of Psychopedagogy after the completion of the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education during the same academic year.

We used a pre-experimental design where pre- and post-treatment measures of the dependent variable were not taken within the same study group; but each group represented a different measure by representing two different formative moments. Thus, we considered PED as the Pre-test measure; the formative process before being PSP as the treatment or Test; and PSP as the Post-test measure.

For the second study (Design II) we established a transversal design, based on the samples of the previous design, in order to form four groups made up of students in a specific formative moment and who had carried out the task of summarizing.

  • Group 1. PED Text: newly enrolled students who had completed the task by reading a printed text.

  • Group 2. PED Hypertext: new students of the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education who carried out the task after reading a hyperlinked text.

  • Group 3. PSP Text: some students of Psychopedagogy who did it after reading a printed text.

  • Group 4. PSP Hypertext: Psychopedagogy students who completed the task after reading a hypertext.

Thus, we used a quasi-experimental methodology with multiple observations of the dependent variable in each of the study groups to replace the control. In order to do this, we took as the Pre-test measure the results in the Design I test obtained by the students who composed the sample of this second design; as Test or treatment, the stimulus of the textual format (written or hyperlinked) when summarizing; and the results obtained in carrying out this task, as a Post-test measure.

Population and sample

The difficulties found in extracting randomly a sample from the study population led us to take three incidental groups as an initial sample: two from Primary Education and one from Psychopedagogy. We had access to them during the first three weeks of the academic year 2013/2014.

As described in Table 1 , the sample for Design I consisted of a total of 130 students, 96 of which participated in the tests exclusively corresponding to Design II. In other words, if we take into account that the design of study I is the Pre-test phase of Design II, once the corresponding tests have been carried out, the sample of this second design was made up of 96 students divided into four groups according to the formative moment they were, and the textual format in which they carried out the tests.

Table 1
Definitive sample for Design I and Design II

Instruments

Table 2 shows the different instruments used to measure the variables considered in the research, as well as the study and the phases these instruments were applied. In the first study we used:

Table 2
Instruments and variables
  • A questionnaire to determine the final composition of the sample, with two clearly differentiated parts and two different objectives: (1) collection of information on demographic data, and (2) on reading habits. In the first part, the aim was to obtain both identifying information on the subjects and academic data on the sample (center of origin, university entrance qualification and enrolment in a different university degree), while the second part was made up of a set of questions regarding the reading habits of the students.

  • A test of basic comprehension and written expression skills to analyze the dependent variable basic comprehension and written expression skills for the construction of knowledge (VD) through 4 sub-variables (see Table 2 ). It was composed of an adapted informative text so that the students had enough time to carry out two proposed tasks: the extraction of key ideas from each textual paragraph and the construction of a conceptual map reflecting the hierarchical relations between the fundamental ideas of the text.

In the second study, for the Pre-test phase, the instruments from the previous study were used to determine the students’ basic comprehension and written expression skills, and to analyze the considered control variables. In the Post-test phase, instruments were used to test whether students were capable of use strategically their comprehension and written expression skills to perform an academic simple task: summarize a document in a printed or hyperlinked format.

In this phase in particular, the dependent variable Specific skills of comprehension and written expression for the synthesis of information (VDTH) was analyzed through two operative variables, using the following instruments:

  • A text-hypertext summary test with which to analyze the operative dependent variable Use of comprehension and written expression skills to perform the task (VDTH1); conformed by another subset of variables related to the textual stimulus presented (see Table 2 ). The test was composed of an academic text about the discipline of Educational Technology, a glossary with the definition of the predictably more confusing terms, and a blank sheet to perform the summary task. The academic article was adapted to be presented in printed format, reducing its length; and in hyperlinked format, establishing a main page and a set of linked nodes.

  • A metacognitive test to study the operative variable Procedures that the student considers to have followed to perform the task (VDTH2) in printed format and hyperlinked.

All the instruments were validated using the expert judgment technique, which included processes of content analysis individually by each expert, and also in groups, so that researchers could check the degree of coincidence and divergence in their evaluations, and discuss them until a high level of consensus was achieved.

Data analysis

Data analysis was carried out in two phases. Firstly, a group of experts analyzed and evaluated the content of the tasks carried out by students, first individually and then in groups, until a high degree of agreement was reached in their judgments. And secondly, different types of descriptive (contingency tables) and inferential (statistical χ2, t-test of mean difference, ANOVA of a factor) statistical analyses were carried out.

Results

We present the results obtained in two different sections to show them clearly. Results obtained in Design I

To study the sub-variable Formation of the main idea of each paragraph (VD1) we analyzed the ideas extracted by the students from each textual paragraph, without appreciating inter-group differences (level of significance, LS=0.05) in any of them (see Table 3 ). After this analysis, we added the scores obtained by each student and established their general level of performance according to the interval in which the total score obtained was situated (between very low and very high). Thus, we observed that the level of students in this sub-variable was between low and very low. The t-test for independent samples confirmed that there were no inter-group statistical differences (t=0.174, p=0.862).

Table 3
Formation of the main idea of each paragraph (VD1)

The difficulties observed when forming the main ideas or keys of each paragraph are specified in Table 4 , where differences can only be seen in paragraph 7. Specifically, 45.5% of Psychopedagogy students misinterpreted the information in that paragraph, while 57.8% of the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education students did not adequately express the information extracted.

Table 4
Difficulties in forming the main ideas or keys of each paragraph (VD2)

In terms of the sub-variable Construction of a conceptual map (VD3), we have to point out that no student was able to reflect the structural relationships between the most important ideas of the text, and only 3.1% structured these ideas around a subordinate organizational form (χ2=0.651; p=0.420).

Regarding the type of Difficulties in the construction of the conceptual map (VD4), it should be noted that 64.3% of the students constructed one in which the overall structure and/or meaning was not clear. Besides, 26.2%, in spite of constructing a clearly structured conceptual map, defined relationships that were not the ones established in the text. Therefore, it is not possible to speak of intergroup differences around this variable (χ2=3.447; p=0.178).

Based on the results and taking into account the variables considered, we observed a quite low level of reading comprehension in general. On the one hand, the level of student performance for the sub-variable Formation of the main idea of each paragraph (VD1) was between low and very low; and on the other hand, 96.9% of the sample presented difficulties with the construction of the conceptual map (Sub-variable VD3).

We must take into account that, as Sánchez et al. (2009 SÁNCHEZ , Emilio ; GARCÍA , José Ricardo ; ROSALES , Javier . Para ayudar a comprender: ¿Qué se debe hacer? ¿Qué se hace? ¿Qué se puede hacer? Aula de Innovación Educativa , Barcelona , v. 180 , p. 65 - 70 , 2009 . , 2010 SÁNCHEZ , Emilio ; GARCÍA , José Ricardo ; ROSALES , Javier . La lectura en el aula: qué se hace, qué se puede hacer, qué se debe hacer . Barcelona : Grao , 2010 . ) pointed out; it is difficult for readers to process the text at the local and global levels simultaneously, since this would imply an important expenditure of cognitive resources. Therefore, they must be able to simplify the task of reading by properly managing their resources in order to move from a microstructural cycle to a macro-structural one. Thus, it is very useful for the reader to know and use techniques such as the extraction of key ideas or the conceptual map to note down on paper both ideas and relations as this allows to recover them when needed, without using the long-term memory.

All the abovementioned difficulties lead us to think that when students are faced with a more complex text, such as, the text/hypertext of the Post-test phase of Design II, they will have problems managing their resources in order to achieve adequate performance.

Results obtained in Design II

Based on the data obtained in Design I for those students who also performed the Post-test phase tests, we elaborated the same analyses in the four groups that made up the sample of this second design.

The analysis of the control variables considered does not show inter-group differences (LS=0.05). Moreover, these differences do not occur in three of the four sub-variables analyzed to assess the Basic skills in comprehension and written expression for knowledge construction (VD). The students reached a general performance level between low and very low in the sub-variable Formation of the main idea of each paragraph (VD1), showing difficulties that, taking into account the sub-variable Difficulties in forming the main idea of each paragraph (VD2), indicate a scarce competence to adequately express the extracted information. It should be noted that 95.8% of the sample also presents difficulties when it comes to constructing the conceptual map.

In the same way as in Design I, the students, in this case organized in four groups, presented scarce Basic skills in comprehension and written expression , as can be seen from the type of performance, between low and very low, in the formation of the main idea of each paragraph (VD1). In addition, they showed difficulties for the Construction of a conceptual map where the macrostructure of the read text is collected (VD3).

We now describe the results obtained in the Post-test phase of the second design. We analyzed the dependent variable Specific skills in comprehension and written expression for information synthesis (VDTH) starting from two operative variables: Use of comprehension and written expression skills to perform the task (VDTH1) and Procedures that the student considers to have followed to perform the task (VDTH2).

As for the first variable (VDTH1), the analyses did not show inter-group differences for most of the sub-variables considered. The sub-variables related to reading printed text indicate that 93.2% of the students did not make notes on it, and 97.7% did not collect information from the footer in the summary they wrote. It is worth mentioning, however, that a significant number of students belonging to the PSP text group highlighted some structural aspects, while those belonging to the PED group performed these tasks in a more diversified manner. On the other hand, the sub-variables related to both the reading of printed text and hyperlinked text, reflect that 94.8% of the students did not make a draft and all of them had difficulties in elaborating the summary, since 86.5% of them built a text with no clear meaning.

Only statistically significant differences appear in the sub-variable Intertextual integration (Hypertext) (VDTH1.3), associated with the fact that 28.5% of the PSP hypertext group used the links, while none of the PED hypertext group did (=10.012; p=.007). Despite these differences, it seems clear that most students do not collect information from hyperlinks in their abstract (88.5%).

Taking all this into consideration, and situating the findings within current studies on the subject, such as those of Escoriza (2006) ESCORIZA , José . Estrategias de comprensión del discurso escrito expositivo: evaluación e intervención . Barcelona : Universidad de Barcelona , 2006 . , Meyer and Ray (2011) MEYER , Bonnie J. F. ; RAY , Melissa N . Structure strategy interventions: Increasing reading comprehension of expository text . International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education , v. 4 , n. 1 , p. 127 - 152 , 2011 . and Van Dijk (2006) VAN DIJK , Teun A . Discourse, context and cognition . Discourse Studies , v. 8 , n. 1 , 159 - 177 , 2006 . ; or within other older studies ( MADLER; JOHNSON, 1977 MADLER , Jean M. ; JOHNSON , Nancy S. Remembrance of things parsed: story structure and recall . Cognitive Psychology , v. 9 , p. 111 - 151 , 1977 . ; VAN DIJK; KINTSCH, 1983 VAN DIJK , Teun ; KINTSCH , Walter . Strategies of discourse comprehension . New York : Academic Press , 1983 . ), we can state that students had problems when strategically using the textual superstructure in order to extract the macrostructure from the text. Thus, they follow synthesis strategies closely linked to the surface code, do not make annotations in the text, barely manage to focus on the structural aspects of the text when underlining, and do not use a draft. We suppose that the main cause of these problems is the lack of adequate training to perform this type of task. In addition to this, those who read the hypertext did not use the information from the hyperlinks in the written summary: due to the difficulties we have just pointed out, it would respond mainly to problems of understanding rather than to the use of structural criteria.

Regarding the Procedures that the student considers to have followed to perform the task (VDTH2), Table 5 shows that there are differences among the groups in all the sub-variables considered. Although most of the students who read the printed text did so once or several times, as well as underlining it, there was 22.6% of the PED text group that did not perform the underlining activity. While 61.5% of the students belonging to the PSP text group summarized from what they had underlined, the majority of the PED text group did so directly from the document (74.2%). A significant proportion of students in the PSP hypertext group read the hyperlinks (71.4%), but 64.5% of those in the PED hypertext group did not. Finally, it should be added that, despite the existing intergroup differences, most of the students summarized directly from the hyperlinked text.

Table 5
Procedures the student claims to follow to complete the task (VDTH2).

As a result, both Psychopedagogy and Primary Education students used reading comprehension procedures that can be included within the category of immature reader. However, as we have shown, there are certain differences in the used procedures when carrying out the task of summarizing the printed document.

Discussion and conclusions

The first conclusion from the results obtained is that the formative moment in the university environment is not a determining factor for the possession of basic skills of reading comprehension and written expression in the sample considered. The analysis of the tasks of extraction of key ideas and construction of a conceptual map indicates that both the students who began the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education, and those who had accessed to the University Degree in Psychopedagogy (second cycle) from the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education, showed important deficits in reading skills such as following the thematic progression of the text, strategically using the textual superstructure to process it globally and self-regulating metacomprehension.

Secondly, textual format is not also a determining variable for predicting a better level of performance when summarizing an academic document in writing. The students, in general, constructed summaries with no clear global meaning, showing deficiencies that indicated the use of synthesis strategies more linked to the surface code of the text than to the construction of the situation model. Furthermore, taking into account that most of the students did not use a draft, it was difficult for them to manage their cognitive resources properly in order to process the text globally. All this seems to indicate that the text/hypertext structure was not strategically used to extract the macro-structure. On the other hand, more specifically, we see that those who read the academic document in printed format did not use techniques such as note-taking, a strategy that would have allowed them to highlight and synthesize relevant information. Although the most used technique was the underlining, only a few of them focused on aspects related to the structure of the text. However, both the students who read the academic document in hyperlinked format and those who read it in printed format did not use the intertextual information in the summary they wrote. If we take into account that the students in general were not able to use the superstructure strategically to extract the macrostructure from the text/hypertext, we consider that the absence of intertextual information in the summaries could be due more to comprehension difficulties than to the use of structural criteria.

According to different authors ( MCNAMARA et al., 2007 MCNAMARA , Danielle S et al . The 4-pronged comprehension strategy framework . In: MCNAMARA , Danielle S . ( Org.) . Reading comprehension strategies: theories, interventions, and technologies Mahwah : Erlbaum , 2007 . p. 465-496. ; MEYER; RAY, 2011 MEYER , Bonnie J. F. ; RAY , Melissa N . Structure strategy interventions: Increasing reading comprehension of expository text . International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education , v. 4 , n. 1 , p. 127 - 152 , 2011 . ; SALMERÓN; GUTIÉRREZ-BRAOJOS, 2012 SALMERÓN , Honorio ; GUTIERREZ-BRAOJOS , Calixto . La competencia de aprender a aprender y el aprendizaje autorregulado . Posicionamientos teóricos. Profesorado , Granada , v. 16 , n. 1 , p. 5 - 13 , 2012 .) mature readers follow a series of procedures: goal setting, textual exploration or reading through in the before-reading stage; taking notes in the text, asking self questions and solving comprehension problems during the reading stage; and drafting in the after-reading stage. Considering all this, as well as the level of performance in the task, it seems evident that the procedures the students used to summarize were not adequate. None of them mentioned setting goals, taking notes in the text, asking oneself questions, solving comprehension problems, or drafting.

Despite this, and considering the differences found (LS=0.05), mainly linked to the formative moment, it is possible to affirm that, in certain aspects, the procedures used by the students of Psychopedagogy were better than those of Primary Education. The process used by the students of Psychopedagogy to summarize with the underlined information resulted more adequate than the one followed by the group of Primary Education, who carried it out directly from the reading of the text. The Psychopedagogy students read at least part of the hyperlinks, while a significant proportion of those of Primary Education did not access any of them. A good part of the Psychopedagogy sample used the glossary, while the majority of the students of Primary Education did not do so despite of the fact that they should be less familiar with the terminology. In general, the students stated that the summary task was very useful, and considered that they had no difficulties in carrying it out. This suggests that, although they knew the importance of the task, the students were not aware of the possible difficulties involved in its elaboration.

In short, it does not seem that the students who had completed the Bachelor’s Degree and were studying the University Degree in Psychopedagogy had acquired, after three years of academic training, effective skills in reading comprehension and written expression. On the contrary: they showed similar difficulties to the students enrolled in the first year of the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education when they had to face tasks such as extracting key ideas, constructing a conceptual map, or preparing a summary. With the results obtained, it is necessary for the educational community in general and educational agents with legislative capacity in particular to carry out an in-depth reflection on the measures to reduce the deficiencies of reading comprehension and written expression in university students. It is worthy to meditate whether educational policies, including universities’ curricula, should be adapted to improve training in these skills. It is paradoxical that certain publications, such as the PISA reports on reading comprehension of 15-year-old students generate great interest and even controversy internationally in the educational communities, whilst it is not usual to find studies on the competence of university students who, as those enrolled in the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education, will carry out their future work as teachers.

It is true that the results obtained at the University of Salamanca cannot be extended to other contexts due to the characteristics of the sample analyzed. However, we consider essential that, both in the initial formative stage (Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education) and in the performance of their professional work, teachers shall receive adequate training in summary writing techniques, extraction of key ideas from texts and hypertexts, as well as in the design of concept maps. To this end, we estimate as necessary, in the first place, to develop programs for students enrolled in Bachelor’s Degrees in Primary Education, so that future teachers learn to use all the abovementioned strategies. These specific training programs must be integrated in the curriculum, and shall be related to communication and technology contents, where the two textual formats considered in this research are used. Secondly, training programs for lecturers of the Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education shall be provided, so lecturers will have the means to train their students in the strategic and self-regulated use of the skills involved in written comprehension and production.

Referencias

  • ARROYO , Rosario ; JIMÉNEZ-BAENA , Abrahan F . Estructuración del ensayo científico sobre contenidos interculturales y competencias escritoras de estudiantes universitarios . Revista de Investigación Educativa , Murcia , v. 34 , n. 2 , p. 351 - 367 , 2016 .
  • CABERO-ALMENARA , Julio ; PIÑERO , Rocío ; REYES , Miguel María . Material educativo multimedia para el aumento de estrategias metacognitivas de comprensión lectora . Perfiles Educativos , Ciudad de México , v. 40 , n. 159 , p. 144 - 159 , 2018 .
  • DUGAREVA , Elena A. ; DIÉGUEZ , Raquel ; PÉREZ , Nereyda . Modelo didáctico del proceso de formación comunicativa científica escrita de los estudiantes universitarios . Pedagogía Universitaria , La Habana , v. 19 , n. 4 , p. 55 - 68 , 2014 .
  • ESCORIZA , José . Estrategias de comprensión del discurso escrito expositivo: evaluación e intervención . Barcelona : Universidad de Barcelona , 2006 .
  • ESPAÑA . Ministerio de Educación, Cultura y Deporte . Programa para la evaluación internacional de los alumnos: informe español . Madrid : Ministerio de Educación, Cultura y Deporte , 2017 .
  • ESPAÑA . Ministerio de Educación, Cultura y Deporte . Resultados del informe PIAAC de la OCDE . Madrid : Ministerio de Educación, Cultura y Deporte , 2013 .
  • HERRADA , Gabriel ; QUINTERO , Anunciación . Habilidades básicas implicadas en la construcción del conocimiento en los alumnos universitarios a través de textos hipervinculados . In: JORNADAS UNIVERSITARIAS DE TECNOLOGÍA EDUCATIVA, 19 ., 2011 , Sevilla . Anales… Sevilla : Universidad de Sevilla , 2011 . p. 778 - 786 .
  • HIRVELA , Alan ; DU , Qian . Why am I paraphrasing: undergraduate ESL writers’ engagement with source-based academic writing and reading . Journal of English for Academic Purposes , v. 12 , n. 2 , p. 87 - 98 , 2013 .
  • KINTSCH , Walter . The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction integration model . Psychological Review , v. 95 , n. 2 , p. 163 - 183 , 1988 .
  • MADLER , Jean M. ; JOHNSON , Nancy S. Remembrance of things parsed: story structure and recall . Cognitive Psychology , v. 9 , p. 111 - 151 , 1977 .
  • MANDIN , Sonia ; LEMAIRE , Benoît ; DESSUS , Philippe . Modeling summarization assessment strategies with LSA . In: EUROPEAN WORKSHOP ON LATENT SEMANTIC ANALYSIS IN TECHNOLOGY ENHANCED LEARNING, 1 ., 2007 , Heerlen . Anales… Heerlen : Open University of Netherlands , 2007 . p. 20 - 21 .
  • MARTÍNEZ , Waltraud ; ESQUIVEL , Ismael . Efectos de la instrucción de estrategias de lectura, mediadas por TIC, en la comprensión lectora del inglés . Perfiles Educativos , Ciudad de México , v. 39 , n. 157 , p. 105 - 122 , 2017 .
  • MCNAMARA , Danielle S et al . The 4-pronged comprehension strategy framework . In: MCNAMARA , Danielle S . ( Org.) . Reading comprehension strategies: theories, interventions, and technologies Mahwah : Erlbaum , 2007 . p. 465-496.
  • MEYER , Bonnie J. F. ; RAY , Melissa N . Structure strategy interventions: Increasing reading comprehension of expository text . International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education , v. 4 , n. 1 , p. 127 - 152 , 2011 .
  • RAMÍREZ , Pamela ; ROSSEL , Katherine ; NAZAR , Gabriela . Comprensión lectora y metacognición: análisis de las actividades de lectura en dos textos de estudio de la asignatura de lenguaje y comunicación de séptimo año básico . Estudios Pedagógicos , Valdivia , v. 41 , n. 2 , p. 213 - 231 , 2015 .
  • RELLO , Javier et al . Funcionamiento ejecutivo y comprensión lectora al finalizar educación primaria . International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology , Badajoz , v. 1 , n. 2 , p. 367 - 376 , 2018 .
  • ROUET , Jean-François ; BRITT , M. Anne . Relevance processes in multiple document comprehension . In: MCCRUDDEN , Matthew , MAGLIANO , Joseph ; SCHRAW , Gregory ( Org .) . Relevance instruction and goal-focusing in text learning . Greenwich : Information Age , 2011 . p. 19 - 52 .
  • RUIZ , Rocío ; VÁZQUEZ , Esteban ; SEVILLANO , María Luisa . La rúbrica de evaluación de la competencia en expresión escrita: percepción del alumnado sobre su funcionalidad . Ocnos , Cuenca , v. 16 , n. 2 , p. 107 - 110 , 2017 .
  • SALMERÓN , Honorio ; GUTIERREZ-BRAOJOS , Calixto . La competencia de aprender a aprender y el aprendizaje autorregulado . Posicionamientos teóricos. Profesorado , Granada , v. 16 , n. 1 , p. 5 - 13 , 2012 .
  • SÁNCHEZ , Emilio ; GARCÍA , José Ricardo ; ROSALES , Javier . La lectura en el aula: qué se hace, qué se puede hacer, qué se debe hacer . Barcelona : Grao , 2010 .
  • SÁNCHEZ , Emilio ; GARCÍA , José Ricardo ; ROSALES , Javier . Para ayudar a comprender: ¿Qué se debe hacer? ¿Qué se hace? ¿Qué se puede hacer? Aula de Innovación Educativa , Barcelona , v. 180 , p. 65 - 70 , 2009 .
  • SÁNCHEZ , Emilio ; SOLÉ , Isabel . Avanzando en la lectura: acuerdos, contrastes y propuestas , Aula de Innovación Educativa , Barcelona , v. 179 , p. 60 - 64 , 2009 .
  • SANELEUTERIO , Elia . El desarrollo de la competencia ortográfica en los estudios de Magisterio . Boletín Redipe , Cali , v. 7 , n. 6 , p. 33 - 38 , 2018 .
  • TIJERO , Talia . Representaciones mentales: discusión crítica del modelo de situación de Kintsch , Onomázein , Santiago do Chile , v. 19 , p. 111 - 138 , 2009 .
  • VAN DIJK , Teun A . Discourse, context and cognition . Discourse Studies , v. 8 , n. 1 , 159 - 177 , 2006 .
  • VAN DIJK , Teun ; KINTSCH , Walter . Strategies of discourse comprehension . New York : Academic Press , 1983 .
  • VEGA , Yuri ; TORRES , Angélica María ; DEL CAMPO , Manuel . Habilidades metamorfológicas y su incidencia en la comprensión lectora . Revista Signos , Valparaíso , v. 50 , n. 95 , p. 453 - 471 , 2017 .
  • WESTBY , Carol et al . Summarizing expository texts . Top Lang Disorders , v. 30 , n. 4 , p. 275 - 287 , 2010 .

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    14 Oct 2019
  • Date of issue
    2019

History

  • Received
    27 May 2018
  • Accepted
    21 Sept 2018
  • Reviewed
    06 Nov 2018
Faculdade de Educação da Universidade de São Paulo Av. da Universidade, 308 - Biblioteca, 1º andar 05508-040 - São Paulo SP Brasil, Tel./Fax.: (55 11) 30913520 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revedu@usp.br