Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

EFFECT OF AUDITORY ENRICHMENT (MUSIC) IN PREGNANT SOWS WELFARE

ABSTRACT

Studies have shown that the benefits from music can improve the welfare conditions of laboratory, pet and production animals. The study aimed to evaluate the influence of music (Antonio Lucio Vivaldi compositions) on behavior aspects (position, activity, stereotypes, social and agonistic behavior) and physiological responses (respiratory rate and rectal temperature) in gestating sows in individual cages (stage 1), and collective stalls (stage 2). 28 animals were studied, 14 belonged to music group, and 14 to the control group (without music).The respiratory rate had the lowest averages in the group with music, for example, in stage 1, time 1, values of 19.28 and 21.50 mov.min−1. In stage 2, the values found in the same time were 20.36 and 24.16, and in time 2, values of 24.37 and 27.18 mov.min−1. These findings follow in stage 2 until time 4.Behaviors position, activity, stereotypes and agonistic interaction with the researcher were also influenced by the music. We concluded from the results that music influenced the behavior of animals, providing greater relaxing and reduce stereotypes aspects related to a better welfare.

KEYWORDS:
animal behavior; physiological parameters; sound stimuli stereotypes

INTRODUCTION

The breeding systems of swine in Brazil are currently mostly intensive, where animals are kept in restricted areas, making it impossible to express their natural behaviors (BAXTER et al., 2011BAXTER, E. M.; LAWRENCE, A. B.; EDWARDS, S. A. Alternative farrowing systems: design criteria for farrowing systems based on the biological needs of sows and piglets. Animal, Cambridge, v.5, n.4, p.580-600. feb. 2011.).Because of this, several researches have been carried out in order to improve the physical, psychological and social conditions of animals raised in confinement and/or captivity. As a technique to improve the biological functioning of the animal created in intensive systems by the modification of the environment, there is the environmental enrichment (COLEMAN et al., 2012COLEMAN, K.; BLOOMSMITH, M.A.; CROCKETT, C.M.; WEED, J.L.; SHAPIRO, S.J. Behavioral management, enrichment, and psychological well-being of laboratory nonhuman primates. In: ABGE, C.R.; MANSFIELD, K.; TARDIF, S.D., MORRIS, T. (Ed). Nonhuman primates in biomedical research. 2nd ed. Elsevier, 2012. v.2, cap.6, p.149-176.).

In recent years, researches have been directed at exploring the impact of sound stimulation as a method of environmental enrichment for captive and confined animals. There are scientific reports that prove the benefits of such stimuli for laboratory animals, pets and some species of production (CRUZ et al., 2011CRUZ, J.G.P.; MAGRO, D.D.; CRUZ, J.N.Efeitos da música clássica como elemento de enriquecimento ambiental em Mus musculus em cativeiro (Rodentia: Muridae). Biotemas, Florianópolis, v.23, n.2, p.191-197, jun. 2011.).The effects of noise in general and music in particular on the behavior and level of animal welfare need to be highlighted in terms of scientific research.

Music can provide benefits by “masking” sudden, artificial noises that are potentially stressful to the animal, and “breaking” the silence or transforming a monotonous audibly environment, known to affect the development of normal cognitive abilities in a pleasant environment and more productive (CRUZ et al., 2011CRUZ, J.G.P.; MAGRO, D.D.; CRUZ, J.N.Efeitos da música clássica como elemento de enriquecimento ambiental em Mus musculus em cativeiro (Rodentia: Muridae). Biotemas, Florianópolis, v.23, n.2, p.191-197, jun. 2011.).

The physiological benefits of listening to music are already well elucidated for humans, which refer to reduction of anxiety, pain relief, and reduction of physiological parameters that, when elevated, may be related to stress, such as blood pressure and heart rate (BINNS-TURNER et al., 2011BINNS-TURNER, P. G.; WILSON, L.L.; PRYOR, E.R.; BOYD, G.L.; PRICKETT, C.A. Perioperative music and its effects on anxiety, hemodynamics, and pain in women undergoing mastectomy. American Association of Nurse Anesthetists Journal, Park Ridge, v.79, n. 4, p.21-27, aug. 2011.), indicating that music could be used to enrich the environment for animals raised in intensive systems.

The value of music as environmental enrichment for animals depends on the musical style used, speed of music, rhythm, tone, frequency, intensity, etc., which will influence the behavioral and physiological responses of the animals (ALWORTH & BUERKLE, 2013ALWORHT, L.C., BUERKLE, S.C. The effects of music on animal physiology, behavior and welfare. Lab animal, New York, v. 42, n.2, p. 54-61, feb. 2013.).

Therefore, this study has the aim of evaluating the interference of auditory sensorial enrichment through music in the level of welfare of sows during the gestation. Within this context, we intend to evaluate specifically: (1) the relation of music with the responses of physiological variables of pregnant females; (2) the relation of music with aspects of the behavior of sows in the gestation stages.

MATERIAL AND METHODS

The experiment was conducted at the Esmeralda farm, whose main activity is the commercial production of swine, located in the municipality of Mogi-Mirim, SP, latitude of 22°32’37”, longitude of 46°58’15” W and altitude of 607m, the climate of the region is considered as mesothermal tropical, according to the Köppen classification (1936).The experimental phase started on June 12th until September 18th, 2014.In order to carry out the research, 28 pregnant sows were studied, followed during the two stages of the study, stage 1 (36 days) referring to the period in which the sows were housed in individual cages, and stage 2 (71 days) referring to the stage that the animals were housed in collective stalls. At each stage, 14 animals were assigned to the group with music (WM) and 14 to the control group (without music - WOM).The animals of the WM group were submitted to the musical repertoire, which consisted of 12 compositions of the composer Antonio Lucio Vivaldi, during two days per week (behavioral evaluation day - BE and physiological evaluation - PE), throughout the study, the control group remained exposed only to sounds coming from their environment.

We decided to perform the behavioral and physiological evaluations on different days, since the act of measuring the rectal temperature of the animals, for example, already influences the animals' behavior. Thus, behavioral evaluations were already being influenced by the manipulation of the animal, especially at the stage where the sows were housed in collective stalls, where the experimenter had to enter the facility. The sound stimulus was reproduced for two consecutive hours, with intervals between the reproductions of two hours; this was done from 8:00 a.m. to 6:00 p.m. For the reproduction of the musical repertoire, two sound boxes were used in both stages, whose sound frequency of the compositions was in the range that covers the range perceived by the swine, which is of 20 Hz – 20.000 Hz (GUSTAFSSON, 1997GUSTAFSSON, B. The health and safety of workers in a confined animal system. Livestock Production Science, Amsterdam, v.49, n.2, p.191-202, sep. 1997.). In order for the sound pressure level to be known and monitored, two digital sound level meter, MSL-1352 C model, Minipa ® brand were used. This measurement was carried out in both groups, with and without music.

The hearing range of swine is similar to that of humans, hearing problems can be caused by exposure to the sound pressure level above 85 dB, and noise above 100 dB was verified during manual feeding of swine and it is suggested that during this task the worker wear ear protector (GUSTAFSSON, 1997GUSTAFSSON, B. The health and safety of workers in a confined animal system. Livestock Production Science, Amsterdam, v.49, n.2, p.191-202, sep. 1997.).

Ethograms were elaborated based on behavioral elements registered in sows housed in individual gestation cages and in collective stalls according to preliminary studies (GONÇALVES et al., 2014GONÇALVES, T.M.; BASTOS, R.; SOARES, R.T.N.; TORRES, D.S. Indicadores comportamentais na avaliação do bem-estar de matrizes de suínos alojadas em baias individuais e piquetes coletivos. Jornal Brasileiro de Ciência Animal, Campos dos Goytacazes, v.7, n.14, p. 523-540, dez. 2014.). The ethograms were adjusted according to the observations made on the farm, consisting of position, activity, stereotypy and agonistic behaviors (Table 1). On BE days (one day per week), observations were made from 8:00 a.m. to 6:00 p.m. Every ten minutes instantaneous sampling of behavior was done. The observations were divided in time ranges, from 8 a.m. to 10 a.m. (1); from 10:10 a.m. to 12h (2); from 12:10 p.m. to 2:00 p.m. (3); from 2:10 p.m. to 4:00 p.m. (4) and from 4:10 p.m. to 6:00 p.m. (5). In the stage 1, 305 behavioral registers were totaled, and in the second stage, 610 observations were recorded.

TABLE 1
Ethogram used in this study to evaluate the behavior of sows.

In the days after the BE, the physiological parameters were measured (respiratory rate and rectal temperature).The records were carried out at the following times: time 1 (from 9:00 a.m.), time 2 (from 11:00 a.m.), time 3 (from 1:00 p.m.), time 4 (from 3:00 p.m.) and time 5 (from 5:00 p.m.). In stage 1, these records totaled 25 collections. In the stage 2, 50 collections were totaled.

To characterize the environment, dry bulb temperature, relative humidity as well as thermal comfort indexes were studied. Climatic variables were recorded throughout the experimental period, using dataloggers. With the record of microclimatic data, the specific enthalpy values of air (kJ/kg of dry air) were calculated according to [eq. (1)], as proposed by RODRIGUES et.al (2010)RODRIGUES, V. C.; SILVA, I.J.; VIEIRA, F.M.; NASCIMENTO, S.T. A correct enthalpy relationship as thermal comfort índex for livestock. International Journal Biometeorology, Heidelberg, v. 55, n. 3, p. 455-459, july. 2010..

(1) h = 1.006. t + R H b p .10 ( 7.5. t 237.3 + t ) . ( 71.28 + 0.052. t )

that,

  • h = Specific enthalpy of air (kJ/kg of dry air);

  • t = Temperature of dry bulb (°C);

  • RH = Air relative humidity (%),

  • bp = Local barometric pressure (considered the value of 706.79mmHg).

The temperature and humidity index (THI) proposed by THOM (1959)THOM, E.C. The discomfort index. Weatherwise, Washington, v.12, n.2, p.57-59, apr. 1959., were also calculated using dry bulb temperature (Dbt) (°C), relative humidity (RH) (%) and dew temperature data (dpt).

(2) T H I = d b t + ( 0.36. d p t ) + 0.42

The data were analyzed by statistical software Minitab17 (2009)MINITAB. Minitab for Windows Release 16. State College: Minitab Inc., 2009.. For the data that did not show normality, we chose to use the Mann-Whitney non-parametric statistical test (P≤0.05). The other data that showed their confirmed normality were submitted to the Tukey test (P <0.05) for comparison of the averages.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Table 2 shows that the environment in which the animals were inserted did not present differences for the averages values of Dbt, RH, H and THI (P> 0.05).

TABLE 2
Average values, standard deviation (SD) of environmental variables for the groups (WM: with music; WOM: without music), according to the stages (1 and 2), in the days of behavioral evaluation (BE) and physiological evaluation (PE).

The averages values of Dbt were higher than the recommended values for pregnant sows (12 to 18°C) according to studies of MOSTAÇO et al. (2014)MOSTAÇO, G.M.; PECORARO, C.A.; SILVA-MIRANDA, K.O. Uso da entalpia como índice de conforto térmico na suinocultura: desenvolvimento e reprodução. Revista Brasileira de Engenharia Agrícola e Ambiental, Campina Grande, 2014, No prelo.. With regard to RH, the averages were outside the recommended standards (60 to 80%) (MOSTAÇO et al., 2014MOSTAÇO, G.M.; PECORARO, C.A.; SILVA-MIRANDA, K.O. Uso da entalpia como índice de conforto térmico na suinocultura: desenvolvimento e reprodução. Revista Brasileira de Engenharia Agrícola e Ambiental, Campina Grande, 2014, No prelo.).

The enthalpy results were within the recommended for pregnant sows, according to the equation proposed by RODRIGUES et al. (2010)RODRIGUES, V. C.; SILVA, I.J.; VIEIRA, F.M.; NASCIMENTO, S.T. A correct enthalpy relationship as thermal comfort índex for livestock. International Journal Biometeorology, Heidelberg, v. 55, n. 3, p. 455-459, july. 2010., with minimum and maximum enthalpy comfort values ranging from 40.80 to 86.57 kJ/kg of dry air. In a study using this same equation, the average value of enthalpy (h) for pregnant sows created in individual cages and collective stalls was 80.01 kJ/kg of dry air (VIEIRA, 2012VIEIRA, R. F. N. Comportamento e desempenho de porcas arraçoadas com diferentes níveis de fibra e criadas em gaiolas ou baias. 2012. 56 f. Dissertação (Mestrado em Zootecnia) - Instituto de Zootecnia, Nova Odessa, 2012.).These data are slightly above the values obtained in this research, but still within the range recommended for swine.

On the other hand, the average value of H, for pregnant sows, corresponding to the period of housing in individual cages and collective stalls was 40.26 kJ/kg of dry air (NUNES, 2011NUNES, M. L. A. Bem-estar de matrizes supinas em gestação: Estimativa da condição de conforto térmico, análise comportamental e produtiva no alojamento coletivo com uso de cama. 2011. 154 f. Tese (Doutorado em Zootecnia) - Escola Superior de Agricultura “Luiz de Queiroz”. Universidade de São Paulo, Piracicaba, 2011.).These data are below the averages values of this study and below the range recommended for pregnant sows. These values may vary according to the conditions of air temperature, relative humidity and local barometric pressure.

When analyzing the values of THI, values that were very close to those considered ideal (70), taking into account that the studies performed with this index are based on researches with dairy cattle. There is a variation in the proposed values of THI in the literature, and HAHN (1985)HAHN, G.L. Management and housing of farm animals in hot environments. In: YOSEF, M.K. (Ed.). Stress physiology in livestock. Boca Raton: CRC PRESS, 1985. chap. 2, p.151-174, 1985. reports that THI values below 70 indicate a normal, non-stressing condition; values between 71 and 78 is critical; between 79 and 83 indicates danger and above 83 already constitutes an emergency situation. At some times, this index was close to 70, and the maximum average value was 71.66, which is located within the lower critical limit.

For the physiological variables, there was no significant difference (P> 0.05) for the rectal temperature. For the respiratory frequency in the first stage, a difference was observed between the groups at time 1 (p <0.05), and in the second stage, significance was found (P <0.05) at times 1, 2, 3 and 4 (Table 3).The average of RF in stage 1 at all times was close to that recommended for pregnant sows (20 mov.min−1), even at times when music was not offered. The statistical difference (P <0.05) between the averages of 19.28 and 21.50 mov.min−1 (WM and WOM), in the first time range, may be related to the presence of music, since the other variables, such as Dbt, RH, H and THI were similar between groups.

TABLE 3
Mean values of respiratory frequency (RF) and rectal temperature (RT) for animals' group: with music (WM) and without music (WOM), according to the stages (1 and 2).

In stage 1, both the animals in the group with music and the animals in the group without music had access to a trough that remained full of water, and at its end the water overflowed at various times of the day, causing the water to flow through the floor, which made it moist. So the animals lay down and the heat exchange by conduction was favored. So in this first stage, this fact may have been important on the responses coming from the values of the respiratory frequencies observed.

In the second stage, the increase of the RF of the sows was consistent throughout the times with decreases of the averages in the time 5.The only time at which the average values were close to the recommended range (15 to 20 mov.min−1) were in the first time (9 a.m.) for the group with music (20.36 mov.min−1). After this period, for both groups the RF was high. In the fourth time (3 p.m.), the highest values of RF, 49.29 and 64 mov.min−1 were observed for the WM and WOM groups. This response was expected due to the fact that this time has shown the higher averages of Dbt and lower values of relative humidity.

In a study with sows housed in individual cages, their RF observed in the times of 9 a.m. and 3 p.m., showed values of 50.6 and 75.4 mov.min−1, in the collective stalls the values of RF were 47.5 and 65.4 mov.min−1, respectively (SANTOS, 2013SANTOS, W.G. Comportamento de matrizes suínas em gestação submetidas a diferentes tipos de alojamentos e condições de sazonalidade. 2013. 81 f. Tese (Doutorado em Zootecnia). Universidade Federal de Viçosa, Viçosa, 2013.).The results presented in the study mentioned above showed higher values than those obtained in this research.

In the first stage, the results indicate that the animals preferentially activated the mechanisms of non-evaporative losses. The animals housed in the collective stalls (step 2) did not have any protection against the sun (propylene mesh, vegetation, etc.), since this side of the shed was exposed to the solar rays throughout the day. So that the evaporative losses happened in this case, which is evidenced by the high respiratory rate values.

The values obtained for RF, which differed significantly (P <0.05) in both stages, showed lower values for the WM group, suggesting that the music may have been able to influence the RF of the animals in certain periods. Such reductions may signal a greater relaxation of the animals in the WM group, and even in the stage 2, where the highest RF averages were observed in both groups, due to the climatic conditions, in the time ranges (1 and 5) where differences were observed, the WM group showed the smaller averages.

In a study evaluating the effects of relaxing music on cancer patients with chronic pain, there was a reduction in blood pressure, heart rate and respiratory rate (NOBRE, et al., 2012NOBRE, D.V.; LEITE, H.R.; ORSINI, M.; CORREA, C.L. Respostas Fisiológicas ao Estímulo Musical: Revisão de Literatura. Revista de Neurociências, São Paulo, v.20, n.4, p. 625-633, ago. 2012.).The rectal temperature response curves were similar in both groups at all times, within each stage. There is a variation throughout the day according to the times, but the averages values are still considered normal for pregnant sows (38.6°C) (HANNAS, 1999HANNAS, M.I. Aspectos fisiológicos e a produção de suínos em clima quente. In: SILVA, I.J.O. (Ed). Ambiência e qualidade na produção industrial de suínos. Piracicaba: Fundação de Estudos Agrários Luiz de Queiroz, 1999. cap.1, p. 1-33.).

The lying position variable (Figure 1) showed a significant difference (P <0.05) in stage 2, in the first range, the values were 80.19 and 66.87% for the WM and WOM groups and in the fifth range; the values were 97.28 and 91.58%, respectively for the WM and WOM groups. In stage 2, the average values over the experimental period for the lying position were 88.74 and 84.87% for the WM and WOM groups, respectively.

FIGURE 1
Relative frequency to the lying position, in stages 1 and 2 for the WM (with music group) and WOM (without music group) over the time ranges.

Lying position variable

The average presented for the stage 2 is above the one found by NUNES (2011)NUNES, M. L. A. Bem-estar de matrizes supinas em gestação: Estimativa da condição de conforto térmico, análise comportamental e produtiva no alojamento coletivo com uso de cama. 2011. 154 f. Tese (Doutorado em Zootecnia) - Escola Superior de Agricultura “Luiz de Queiroz”. Universidade de São Paulo, Piracicaba, 2011., who verified in 77.1% of the observations of pregnant sows in the lying posture. This value is below the averages results of this study, for this variable in the stage 2.

The high percentage of the expression of this behavior, especially in the stage 2 may be associated with room temperature, because in most of the observed periods the Dbt was out the comfort zone. The increase of heat in the environment could promote the prostration of the females in an attempt to optimize the heat losses. The increase of the ambient temperature is associated with the increase of the frequency of swine in the lying position (KIEFER et al., 2012KIEFER, C.; MARTINS, L.P.; FANTINI, C.C. Evaporative cooling for lactating sows under high ambient temperature. Revista Brasileira de Zootecnia, Viçosa, MG, v.41, n.5, p.1180-1185, mai. 2012.).

Concerning the behavior of the standing variable, the difference (P <0.05) was observed only in the stage 2, in the first time range (from 8 a.m. to 10 a.m.) with values of 19.81 (WM) and 29.78% (WOM), and in the fifth time range with an average of 2.55 and 5.44% for WM and WOM groups. The time range 1 showed the highest percentages of standing behavior in both stages, due to the fact that in the morning, between 7 a.m. and 7:30 a.m. the supply of feed happened, after that there was a period of the animals' agitation that lasted until 10 a.m., followed by a reduction of this behavior. The averages along the time ranges for the standing variable in stage 1 were 20.60 (WM) and 22.81% (WOM), in stage 2, the average values were 9.09 (WM) and 11.41% (WOM).

For PANDORFI (2005)PANDORFI, H. Comportamento bioclimático de matrizes suínas em gestação e o uso de sistemas inteligentes na caracterização do ambiente produtivo: suinocultura de precisão. 2005. 137 f. Tese (Doutorado em Física do Ambiente Agrícola) - Escola Superior de Agricultura “Luiz de Queiroz”, Universidade de São Paulo, Piracicaba, 2005., sows in individual cages showed frequency of standing position for 7.87% of the observed times, while those housed in collective stalls had 13.43% of reported frequency. The values mentioned for this variable in the individual gestation are inferior to the results of the stage 1, but in the stage 2, the data are close to the results of this research.

The sitting variable differed (P <0.05) in the stage 1, in the time range 3 (between 12:10 p.m. and 2:00 p.m.) with 1.48 and 4.88%, for the groups WM and WOM, and in the stage 2 in the first (0.00 and 3.35%) and in the range 5 (0.17 and 3.98%), always the lowest values coming from the WM group. The averages over the period for the stage 1 were 3.36 and 5.25%, for the WM and WOM groups, and in the stage 2, the average values were 2.43 and 3.71% for the WM and WOM groups. Aiming to characterize behavioral aspects of pregnant sows, the average frequencies of the sitting position were observed for female swine housed in individual cages and in collective stalls, resulting in 4.40% and 0.58% for each type of housing (PANDORFI, 2005PANDORFI, H. Comportamento bioclimático de matrizes suínas em gestação e o uso de sistemas inteligentes na caracterização do ambiente produtivo: suinocultura de precisão. 2005. 137 f. Tese (Doutorado em Física do Ambiente Agrícola) - Escola Superior de Agricultura “Luiz de Queiroz”, Universidade de São Paulo, Piracicaba, 2005.).Staying seated or standing inactive for long periods may indicate poor welfare. The lying position, on the other hand, may reflect a good welfare situation in the case of sows housed in collective stalls (McGLONE, 2013McGLONE, J.J. Review: Updated scientific evidence on the welfare of gestating sows kept in different housing systems. The Professional Animal Scientist, Savoy, v.29, n.3, p.189-198, jun. 2013.). The behaviors of inactive activity (Figure 2), alert and sniffing showed statistical differences (P <0.05) in both stages. In the stage 1, for the inactive variable, the first time range showed averages of 32 and 21.14%, and in the time range 3, values of 75.50 and 58.62%.In the stage 2, the range 1 showed 59.57 and 33.06%, the range 3 obtained 77.91 and 63.44% and the range 5 with 87.21 and 66.39%.

FIGURE 2
Percentage of inactive behavior for the groups with music (WM) and without music (WOM) for the stage 1 (A) and second stage (B) along the times ranges.

Inactive activity variable

The highest frequencies of inactive behavior are always from the WM group. The averages of this inactive behavior throughout the stage 1, taking into account all the time ranges, were 55.89 and 46.20% for the WM and WOM group. In the stage 2, the averages were 72.67 and 58.32% for the WM and WOM groups. The inactive behavior of pregnant sows in collective stalls was observed in 78.6% of the time in the study of SANTOS (2013)SANTOS, W.G. Comportamento de matrizes suínas em gestação submetidas a diferentes tipos de alojamentos e condições de sazonalidade. 2013. 81 f. Tese (Doutorado em Zootecnia). Universidade Federal de Viçosa, Viçosa, 2013..In addition to the question of the area available per animal, for animals housed in collective stalls, other factors may be related to the differences found, for example, climatic conditions, substrate supply, observation methodology, among others.

The alert activity, in stage 1, was different in the third time range (10.71 and 16.44%); in the second stage, this difference was found in the first range (20.12 and 28.49%), in the third range (13.22 and 22.24%) and in the fifth range (6.59 and 15.73%).The smallest values are always obtained from the WM group. In the stage 1, the presented averages for this variable over the observed period were 19.20 (WM) and 21.94% (WOM).The alert behavior percentage of sows housed in individual cages was 14.6% (SANTOS, 2013SANTOS, W.G. Comportamento de matrizes suínas em gestação submetidas a diferentes tipos de alojamentos e condições de sazonalidade. 2013. 81 f. Tese (Doutorado em Zootecnia). Universidade Federal de Viçosa, Viçosa, 2013.). These results are closer to the values showed by the WM group. In the stage 2, the averages over the period were 15.33 and 21.86% for the WM and WOM groups. When the percentage of the alert behavior of sows housed in collective bays was observed, there was an average value of 11.3% (SANTOS, 2013SANTOS, W.G. Comportamento de matrizes suínas em gestação submetidas a diferentes tipos de alojamentos e condições de sazonalidade. 2013. 81 f. Tese (Doutorado em Zootecnia). Universidade Federal de Viçosa, Viçosa, 2013.). The values of this variable for the WM group are closer to the results of the cited study.

For the sniffing variable, the significant difference (P <0.05) in the first stage was observed in the time range 3, with values of 0.7 and 2.67%, and in the second stage, in range 1, values of 2.63 and 6.98% and in the fifth range, 0.57 and 2.53%.With the lowest values relative to the WM group. The act of sniffing or looking is considered a sign of exploitation of the environment, which in situations of confinement appears much inferior compared to the expression presented by the animals in their natural environment (MAIA et al., 2013MAIA, A.P.A.; SARUBBI, J.; LEMOS, B.B.; MOURA, D.J. Enriquecimento ambiental como medida para o bem-estar positivo de suínos. Revista Eletronica em Gestão, Educação e Tecnologia Ambiental, Santa Maria, v. 14, n. 4, p. 2862-2877, set. 2013.).The presented results regarding the sniffing behavior showed little frequency in this research, probably due to the restricted area at the disposition of the animals and absence of substrate for manipulation.

In general, the WM animals were more inactive, consequently less alert and less likely to perform other activities. This situation may indicate that the animals of the WM group were more relaxed than the animals in the control group. Classical music played for dogs from a shelter showed that the animals spent more time sleeping, less time vocalizing and reduced body tremor when compared to animals exposed to another style. This shows that classical music can mitigate stress situations (KOGAN, et al., 2012KOGAN, L. R.; SCHOENFELD-TACHER, R.; SIMON, A. Behavioral effects of auditory stimulation on kenneled dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, New York, v.7, n.5, p. 268-275, sep./oct. 2012.).

When evaluating the stereotypy data, observed in stage 1, between the WM and WOM groups, the false chewing (FC) behaviors (12.01 and 16.88%) differed statistically (P <0.05) in the first time range and biting the bar (BB) in the second (0.52 and 2.39%) and fifth time range (1.43 and 3.19%), where the lowest observed values were from the WM group. The average values over the period studied in stage 1 of the FC behavior were 6.68% (WM) and 9.42% (WOM) and BB showed values of 1.83 and 3.43% for the groups with and without music. In sows housed in individual cages, values referring to the stereotypy FC and BB were 15.07 and 7.5%, (BERG, 2014BERG, K.C. van den. Effect of cage enrichment on the stereotypic behaviour of commercially kept pregnant sows. 2014. 171 f. Thesis (Doctoral in Biosystems) – Universiteit Utrecht, Utrecht, 2014.).

In the second stage, the stereotyped looking/licking behavior (L/L) differed (P <0.05) in the first time range (8.79 and 17.68%), FC in the first one (5.80 and 8.93 %), in the third (2.70 and 5.97%) and in the fifth time range (4.75 and 8.16%), and for the BB stereotypy, the difference was found in the first time range (0,00 and 1.13%), always the lowest frequencies are from the WM group. The averages over the period in stage 2 of the L/L behavior were 2.49 and 4.85% for the groups with and without music, FC showed an average of 4.53 and 7.09% for the WM and WOM groups and for BB the average values were 0.06 and 0.41% for the WM and WOM groups, respectively. Studying the behavior of sows housed in collective stalls, PANDORFI (2005)PANDORFI, H. Comportamento bioclimático de matrizes suínas em gestação e o uso de sistemas inteligentes na caracterização do ambiente produtivo: suinocultura de precisão. 2005. 137 f. Tese (Doutorado em Física do Ambiente Agrícola) - Escola Superior de Agricultura “Luiz de Queiroz”, Universidade de São Paulo, Piracicaba, 2005. found an average frequency of 9.94% for the L/L stereotypy.

Regarding the FC behavior, the average value was 12.48% of the observed times, throughout the gestation period in collective stalls (VIEIRA, 2012VIEIRA, R. F. N. Comportamento e desempenho de porcas arraçoadas com diferentes níveis de fibra e criadas em gaiolas ou baias. 2012. 56 f. Dissertação (Mestrado em Zootecnia) - Instituto de Zootecnia, Nova Odessa, 2012.).The average values of this stereotypy were 8.07% in sows housed in collective stalls (NUNES, 2011NUNES, M. L. A. Bem-estar de matrizes supinas em gestação: Estimativa da condição de conforto térmico, análise comportamental e produtiva no alojamento coletivo com uso de cama. 2011. 154 f. Tese (Doutorado em Zootecnia) - Escola Superior de Agricultura “Luiz de Queiroz”. Universidade de São Paulo, Piracicaba, 2011.).The practice of looking the floor without the presence of substrates and false chewing evidences the need of the swine to explore the environment in the search for food (RUSHEN, 1984RUSHEN, J. Stereotyped behaviour, adjunctive drinking and the feeding periods of tethered sows. Animal Behaviour, London, v. 32, n. 4, p.1059-1067, nov. 1984.).The grinding of the teeth behavior did not differ statistically (P <0.05), however, such stereotypy was a finding that did not obtain reports in the literature for production animals. The studies found refer to research with laboratory animals, and the most commonly found name is bruxism. Rats that had a stressful situation showed this behavior when compared to animals that were not submitted to such situation. The authors proposed that emotional stress might have led to bruxism (LV et al., 2002).An explanation for this behavior is that it may be related to the psychological stress that the animals may be subjected to, because they are in confinement, as the other stereotypes are a consequence of the frustration, the grinding of the teeth can also be another way of the animals express this condition.

When the averages of all stereotypes at each stage were verified, for the group with and without music, the data showed values of 19.64 and 26.16% in stage 1. In the stage 2, these values were 7.74 for the WM group and 14.13% for the WOM group. In the both stages, the highest frequency of observations regarding stereotyped behaviors was for the group without music, indicating that the music may have been able to minimize its expression.

Social and agonistic behaviors (Table 4) were only observed in the stage 2 and no significant differences were observed (P> 0.05).In relation to the agonistic interaction of the animals in front of the researcher, there was a significant difference (P <0.05) at all times, with the WOM group showing this behavior, this was observed at the moments when it was necessary to enter the bays to measure the RT of the animals. This behavior consisted of one or more animals directing aggressive behaviors against the researcher, such as biting and pulling part of the clothing, boots, and sometimes the skin. The researcher should not act in order to interfere the animal behavior, however, there was no other way to measure the rectal temperature of the animals housed in collective stalls without the entrance of the same in the facility, which made possible the expression of such behavior in the animals, specifically from the control animals.

TABLE 4
Relative frequency (%) of social and agonistics behaviors, and absolute frequency of the agonistic interaction behavior with researcher, in both groups (WM and WOM), by time, in stage 2.

For the sound pressure differences between the time ranges (during behavioral evaluation days) and times (physiological evaluation days) were detected for the WM group, since in this group musical reproduction was intermittent, and this altered the sound pressure according to the time and time range. This situation was observed in the time ranges and times 1, 3 and 5, when the group with music showed higher levels of sound pressure, both for the behavioral and physiological evaluation days, because those were the moments that the musical repertoire was reproduced. The average maximum value of 71.13 dB was from the WM group, during a period when the music was being played, even during the whole musical reproduction period, the level of 85 dB was not exceeded.

CONCLUSIONS

The animals in the group with music in both stages (stage 1: animals housed in individual cages; and stage 2: animals housed in collective stalls), showed in several moments lower values of respiratory frequency, higher percentage of relaxation behaviors, smaller expressions of stereotypes, and absence of agonistic interaction against the human presence compared to the group of sows without music.

REFERENCES

  • ALWORHT, L.C., BUERKLE, S.C. The effects of music on animal physiology, behavior and welfare. Lab animal, New York, v. 42, n.2, p. 54-61, feb. 2013.
  • BAXTER, E. M.; LAWRENCE, A. B.; EDWARDS, S. A. Alternative farrowing systems: design criteria for farrowing systems based on the biological needs of sows and piglets. Animal, Cambridge, v.5, n.4, p.580-600. feb. 2011.
  • BERG, K.C. van den. Effect of cage enrichment on the stereotypic behaviour of commercially kept pregnant sows 2014. 171 f. Thesis (Doctoral in Biosystems) – Universiteit Utrecht, Utrecht, 2014.
  • BINNS-TURNER, P. G.; WILSON, L.L.; PRYOR, E.R.; BOYD, G.L.; PRICKETT, C.A. Perioperative music and its effects on anxiety, hemodynamics, and pain in women undergoing mastectomy. American Association of Nurse Anesthetists Journal, Park Ridge, v.79, n. 4, p.21-27, aug. 2011.
  • COLEMAN, K.; BLOOMSMITH, M.A.; CROCKETT, C.M.; WEED, J.L.; SHAPIRO, S.J. Behavioral management, enrichment, and psychological well-being of laboratory nonhuman primates. In: ABGE, C.R.; MANSFIELD, K.; TARDIF, S.D., MORRIS, T. (Ed). Nonhuman primates in biomedical research 2nd ed. Elsevier, 2012. v.2, cap.6, p.149-176.
  • CRUZ, J.G.P.; MAGRO, D.D.; CRUZ, J.N.Efeitos da música clássica como elemento de enriquecimento ambiental em Mus musculus em cativeiro (Rodentia: Muridae). Biotemas, Florianópolis, v.23, n.2, p.191-197, jun. 2011.
  • GONÇALVES, T.M.; BASTOS, R.; SOARES, R.T.N.; TORRES, D.S. Indicadores comportamentais na avaliação do bem-estar de matrizes de suínos alojadas em baias individuais e piquetes coletivos. Jornal Brasileiro de Ciência Animal, Campos dos Goytacazes, v.7, n.14, p. 523-540, dez. 2014.
  • GUSTAFSSON, B. The health and safety of workers in a confined animal system. Livestock Production Science, Amsterdam, v.49, n.2, p.191-202, sep. 1997.
  • HAHN, G.L. Management and housing of farm animals in hot environments. In: YOSEF, M.K. (Ed.). Stress physiology in livestock Boca Raton: CRC PRESS, 1985. chap. 2, p.151-174, 1985.
  • HANNAS, M.I. Aspectos fisiológicos e a produção de suínos em clima quente. In: SILVA, I.J.O. (Ed). Ambiência e qualidade na produção industrial de suínos Piracicaba: Fundação de Estudos Agrários Luiz de Queiroz, 1999. cap.1, p. 1-33.
  • KIEFER, C.; MARTINS, L.P.; FANTINI, C.C. Evaporative cooling for lactating sows under high ambient temperature. Revista Brasileira de Zootecnia, Viçosa, MG, v.41, n.5, p.1180-1185, mai. 2012.
  • KOGAN, L. R.; SCHOENFELD-TACHER, R.; SIMON, A. Behavioral effects of auditory stimulation on kenneled dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, New York, v.7, n.5, p. 268-275, sep./oct. 2012.
  • LV, X.; LI, Q.; WU, S.; SUN, J.; ZHANG, M., CHEN, Y. J. Psychological stress alters the ultrastructure and increases IL-1β and TNF-α in mandibular condylar cartilage. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, Ribeirão Preto, v. 45, n. 10, p. 968-976, oct. 2012.
  • MAIA, A.P.A.; SARUBBI, J.; LEMOS, B.B.; MOURA, D.J. Enriquecimento ambiental como medida para o bem-estar positivo de suínos. Revista Eletronica em Gestão, Educação e Tecnologia Ambiental, Santa Maria, v. 14, n. 4, p. 2862-2877, set. 2013.
  • McGLONE, J.J. Review: Updated scientific evidence on the welfare of gestating sows kept in different housing systems. The Professional Animal Scientist, Savoy, v.29, n.3, p.189-198, jun. 2013.
  • MINITAB. Minitab for Windows Release 16 State College: Minitab Inc., 2009.
  • MOSTAÇO, G.M.; PECORARO, C.A.; SILVA-MIRANDA, K.O. Uso da entalpia como índice de conforto térmico na suinocultura: desenvolvimento e reprodução. Revista Brasileira de Engenharia Agrícola e Ambiental, Campina Grande, 2014, No prelo.
  • NOBRE, D.V.; LEITE, H.R.; ORSINI, M.; CORREA, C.L. Respostas Fisiológicas ao Estímulo Musical: Revisão de Literatura. Revista de Neurociências, São Paulo, v.20, n.4, p. 625-633, ago. 2012.
  • NUNES, M. L. A. Bem-estar de matrizes supinas em gestação: Estimativa da condição de conforto térmico, análise comportamental e produtiva no alojamento coletivo com uso de cama 2011. 154 f. Tese (Doutorado em Zootecnia) - Escola Superior de Agricultura “Luiz de Queiroz”. Universidade de São Paulo, Piracicaba, 2011.
  • PANDORFI, H. Comportamento bioclimático de matrizes suínas em gestação e o uso de sistemas inteligentes na caracterização do ambiente produtivo: suinocultura de precisão 2005. 137 f. Tese (Doutorado em Física do Ambiente Agrícola) - Escola Superior de Agricultura “Luiz de Queiroz”, Universidade de São Paulo, Piracicaba, 2005.
  • RODRIGUES, V. C.; SILVA, I.J.; VIEIRA, F.M.; NASCIMENTO, S.T. A correct enthalpy relationship as thermal comfort índex for livestock. International Journal Biometeorology, Heidelberg, v. 55, n. 3, p. 455-459, july. 2010.
  • RUSHEN, J. Stereotyped behaviour, adjunctive drinking and the feeding periods of tethered sows. Animal Behaviour, London, v. 32, n. 4, p.1059-1067, nov. 1984.
  • SANTOS, W.G. Comportamento de matrizes suínas em gestação submetidas a diferentes tipos de alojamentos e condições de sazonalidade 2013. 81 f. Tese (Doutorado em Zootecnia). Universidade Federal de Viçosa, Viçosa, 2013.
  • THOM, E.C. The discomfort index. Weatherwise, Washington, v.12, n.2, p.57-59, apr. 1959.
  • VIEIRA, R. F. N. Comportamento e desempenho de porcas arraçoadas com diferentes níveis de fibra e criadas em gaiolas ou baias 2012. 56 f. Dissertação (Mestrado em Zootecnia) - Instituto de Zootecnia, Nova Odessa, 2012.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Mar-Apr 2017

History

  • Received
    29 Mar 2016
  • Accepted
    17 Oct 2016
Associação Brasileira de Engenharia Agrícola SBEA - Associação Brasileira de Engenharia Agrícola, Departamento de Engenharia e Ciências Exatas FCAV/UNESP, Prof. Paulo Donato Castellane, km 5, 14884.900 | Jaboticabal - SP, Tel./Fax: +55 16 3209 7619 - Jaboticabal - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistasbea@sbea.org.br