Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The use of a neck brace does not influence visual vertical perception

O uso do colar cervical não influência na percepção de verticalidade visual

Abstracts

Subjective visual vertical (SVV) evaluates the individual's capacity to determine the vertical orientation. Using a neck brace (NB) allow volunteers' heads fixation to reduce cephalic tilt during the exam, preventing compensatory ocular torsion and erroneous influence on SVV result. OBJECTIVE: To analyze the influence of somatosensory inputs caused by a NB on the SVV. METHOD: Thirty healthy volunteers performed static and dynamic SVV: six measures with and six without the NB. RESULTS: The mean values for static SVV were -0.075º±1.15º without NB and -0.372º±1.21º with NB. For dynamic SVV in clockwise direction were 1.73º±2.31º without NB and 1.53º±1.80º with NB. For dynamic SVV in counterclockwise direction was -1.50º±2.44º without NB and -1.11º±2.46º with NB. Differences between measurements with and without the NB were not statistically significant. CONCLUSION: Although the neck has many sensory receptors, the use of a NB does not provide sufficient afferent input to change healthy subjects' perception of visual verticality.

visual perception; proprioception; saccule; utricle


A subjetiva vertical visual (SVV) avalia a capacidade do indivíduo determinar a posição vertical. O uso do colar cervical (CC) fixa a cabeça do voluntário, reduzindo a inclicação cefálica durante o exame e prevenindo a torção ocular compensatória que influencia na SVV. OBJETIVO: Analisar a influência de informações sometossensoriais causadas pelo uso de um CC na SVV. MÉTODO: Trinta voluntários saudáveis realizaram a SVV estática e dinâmica: seis medidas com e seis medidas sem o CC. RESULTADOS: O valor médio da SVV estática foi -0,075º±1,15º sem CC e -0,372º±1,21º com. Na SVV dinâmica no sentido horário foi 1,73º±2,31º sem CC e 1,53º±1,80º com. Na SVV dinâmica no sentido anti-horário foi -1,50º±2,44º sem CC e -1,11º±2,46º com. As diferenças entre as medidas com e sem CC não foram estatisticamente significativas. CONCLUSÃO: Apesar de o pescoço possuir inumeros receptores sensoriais, o uso do CC não fornece aferências suficientes para alterar a percepção de visual de verticalidade de voluntários saudáveis.

percepção visual; propriocepção; sáculo; utrículo


ARTICLE

The use of a neck brace does not influence visual vertical perception

O uso do colar cervical não influência na percepção de verticalidade visual

Martha FunabashiI; Natya N.L. SilvaII; Luciana M. WatanabeII; Taiza E.G. Santos-PontelliI; José Fernando ColafêminaIII; Antonio A.O. CarneiroIV; Osvaldo M. TakayanaguiI

IDepartment of Neuroscience and Behavior, University of São Paulo, Faculty of Medicine at Ribeirão Preto, Ribeirão Preto SP, Brazil

IIDepartment of Biomechanics, Medicine and Rehabilitation of the Locomotor Apparatus, University of São Paulo, Faculty of Medicine at Ribeirão Preto, Ribeirão Preto SP, Brazil

IIIDepartment of Ophthalmology, Otolaryngology and Head and Neck Surgery, University of São Paulo Faculty of Medicine at Ribeirão Preto, Ribeirão Preto SP Brazil

IVDepartment of Physics and Mathematics, Faculty of Philosophy, Sciences and Letters at Ribeirão Preto, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto SP, Brazil

Correspondence Correspondence: Taiza E.G. Santos-Pontelli Department of Neuroscience and Behavior - University of São Paulo School of Medicine at Ribeirão Preto 14049-900 Ribeirão Preto SP - Brasil E-mail: taiza@fmrp.usp.br

ABSTRACT

Subjective visual vertical (SVV) evaluates the individual's capacity to determine the vertical orientation. Using a neck brace (NB) allow volunteers' heads fixation to reduce cephalic tilt during the exam, preventing compensatory ocular torsion and erroneous influence on SVV result.

OBJECTIVE: To analyze the influence of somatosensory inputs caused by a NB on the SVV.

METHOD: Thirty healthy volunteers performed static and dynamic SVV: six measures with and six without the NB.

RESULTS: The mean values for static SVV were –0.075º±1.15º without NB and –0.372º±1.21º with NB. For dynamic SVV in clockwise direction were 1.73º±2.31º without NB and 1.53º±1.80º with NB. For dynamic SVV in counterclockwise direction was –1.50º±2.44º without NB and –1.11º±2.46º with NB. Differences between measurements with and without the NB were not statistically significant.

CONCLUSION: Although the neck has many sensory receptors, the use of a NB does not provide sufficient afferent input to change healthy subjects' perception of visual verticality.

Key words: visual perception, proprioception, saccule, utricle.

RESUMO

A subjetiva vertical visual (SVV) avalia a capacidade do indivíduo determinar a posição vertical. O uso do colar cervical (CC) fixa a cabeça do voluntário, reduzindo a inclicação cefálica durante o exame e prevenindo a torção ocular compensatória que influencia na SVV.

OBJETIVO: Analisar a influência de informações sometossensoriais causadas pelo uso de um CC na SVV.

MÉTODO: Trinta voluntários saudáveis realizaram a SVV estática e dinâmica: seis medidas com e seis medidas sem o CC.

RESULTADOS: O valor médio da SVV estática foi –0,075º±1,15º sem CC e –0,372º±1,21º com. Na SVV dinâmica no sentido horário foi 1,73º±2,31º sem CC e 1,53º±1,80º com. Na SVV dinâmica no sentido anti-horário foi –1,50º±2,44º sem CC e –1,11º±2,46º com. As diferenças entre as medidas com e sem CC não foram estatisticamente significativas.

CONCLUSÃO: Apesar de o pescoço possuir inumeros receptores sensoriais, o uso do CC não fornece aferências suficientes para alterar a percepção de visual de verticalidade de voluntários saudáveis.

Palavras-chave: percepção visual, propriocepção, sáculo, utrículo.

The ability of humans to spatially orient in relation to the Earth's gravitational axis is important for the maintenance of posture, gait and for most motor activities. This spatial orientation is achieved through four different sensory inputs: the interoceptive, visual, somatosensory and vestibular systems1-7.

The perception of verticality is represented by the subjective haptic vertical (SHV), the subjective postural vertical (SPV) and the subjective visual vertical (SVV). The SHV is determined by manipulating a wooden or metal rod into the Earth-vertical position with the patients' eyes closed and is essentially driven by proprioceptive afferent signals8. The SPV is assessed by seating the subjects in a tiltable chair. Vision is blocked and the subjects must state when they perceive that their body is vertically oriented8-10; this perception is determined only by interoceptive inputs9,11. The SVV is assessed by instructing subjects to adjust a visible line in complete darkness without any references about the real vertical position. This perception depends only on vestibular information with the assistance of the visual clues, independent not only of the somatosensory receptors but also of truncal graviceptors when subjects are in an Earth-vertical position11-13. According to Curthoys et al.14, the change in torsional eye position (following a neurectomy) is accompanied by a change in the perceived visual orientation of a small illuminated line at a straight ahead eye level position in an otherwise completely darkened room.

It has been consistently observed in the literature that the somatosensory inputs influence both the E- and A-effect of SVV. The E-effect is a deviation of the SVV opposite to the head tilt side, when the roll-tilt of the head is less than 45 degrees15,16; the A-effect is a deviation of the SVV in the same side to the head tilt, when the roll-tilt of the head is 80-90 degrees. In the A-effect, even normal subjects invariably set the line tilted in the direction of body tilt by some 10-30 degrees16,17.

Despite the existence of several studies analyzing the SVV in both healthy subjects and vestibular dysfunction patients2,3,18-21, most of them did not ensure fixation of the volunteers' heads. This procedure (which can be achieved with the use of a neck brace) is essential to reduce cephalic tilt during the exam and to minimize possible A- and E-effects that can generate compensatory ocular torsion and influence the SVV result14-17. Based on these observations, the aim of this study was to determine whether the somatosensory inputs that are provided by the use of the neck brace during SVV tests influence the exam's result.

METHOD

Subjects

Thirty healthy volunteers, 23 female (76.7%), aged between 20 and 35 years (mean age 24.17±3.9) were selected. The exclusion criteria included history of any of the following: vestibulopathy (or any previous sensation of dizziness or vertigo), migraine, neurologic and metabolic disease. Subjects who wore visual corrective lenses performed the exam while wearing them.

This study was approved by the University of São Paulo's ethics committee (Comitê de Ética em Pesquisa - CEP, protocol number 364/2008). Written informed consent was obtained from all of the subjects.

Equipment

To assess the SVV, subjects were seated in a 45-cm tall seat and a 30-cm long dark tube was used to isolate the volunteer from visual references. On the white background of a HP Pavilion 15.4' computer, a 11-cm red line was projected for the static SVV7.

The dynamic SVV was assessed using 1- to 5-cm diameter black circles, randomly distributed on a white background, which could move in either a clockwise or a counterclockwise direction at a velocity of 30º/second7.

Procedure

The volunteer remained in a seated upright position, with the right hand on the table to control the computer mouse. The right mouse button could move the line in a clockwise direction and the left mouse button could move it in a counterclockwise direction. The angular deviations of the virtual line were compared to the real vertical and defined as positive if tilted clockwise and negative if tilted in a counterclockwise direction. To minimize the learning effect, each volunteer performed five static SVV measures that were not included in the results of this study. In each session, six measures were performed with the neck brace and six without it. The final result was determined by the mean value of the six measurements in each condition (with and without a neck brace). The exams were performed in the following order: static SVV, clockwise dynamic SVV and counterclockwise dynamic SVV. Between exams, the volunteers rested for five minutes. Furthermore, fifteen volunteers first performed the exam with the neck brace and the other fifteen first performed the exam without the neck brace.

Data analysis

Comparisons of the mean values of the six measures, with and without the neck brace, were performed with SPSS (Statistical Package for Social Sciences) Software 17.0 for Windows. After performing a Shapiro-Wilk test, the variables of static SVV with and without the neck brace and clockwise dynamic SVV with and without the neck brace presented a normal distribution and were analyzed with the Student's t-test, whereas the variables of the counterclockwise dynamic SVV did not present a normal distribution and were analyzed with the Mann-Whitney U test. In all tests, the criterion for statistical significance was two-tailed and set at α<0.05.

RESULTS

Figure presents the median values and the interquartile ranges of each SVV condition with and without neck brace: static SVV, clockwise dynamic SVV and counterclockwise dynamic SVV.


During static SVV, the mean value was very close to the real vertical position (0º) in both conditions, with the neck brace (–0.372º±1.21º) and without it (–0.075º±1.15º). The mean values for dynamic SVV in the clockwise direction were 1.73º±2.31º without a NB and 1.53º±1.80º with a NB. The dynamic SVV in the counterclockwise direction was –1.50º±2.44º without a NB and –1.11º±2.46º with a NB. The differences between the measurements with and without a NB were not statistically significant.

DISCUSSION

Recently, new methods of vestibular system evaluation have been introduced in clinical routines, transforming the investigation of vestibule-ocular reflexes that have originated on otolithic macula15. Thus, more thoroughly understanding the functionality of the otolith end organs leads to a more precise diagnosis and consequently to a better therapeutic response. Among these assessments, the determination of the SVV is a simple and low-cost assay of otolithic function15.

Normal values of static SVV in the Brazilian population vary from –2.0º to +2.4º, independent of age and gender5. In the present study, the volunteers exhibited a performance level that can be considered normal, both with and without the neck brace. Although the dynamic SVV results were slightly higher than the static SVV results, the dynamic SVV deviations in clockwise and counterclockwise directions did not exceed the normal limits for static SVV that were found previously5.

The present study shows that the perception of verticality in healthy volunteers is not affected by somatosensory inputs originating from the use of a neck brace. This result corroborates the findings of Faralli et al.18, who studied the influence of somatosensory inputs that are provided by the plantar surface (like the neck, the plantar surface is also full of mechanoreceptors and tactile receptors). Faralli et al.18 found no significant difference in the SVV results in healthy subjects, when comparing the performance with and without proprioceptive inputs from the plantar surface. These data suggest that the otolithic organs are themselves capable of providing the information that is necessary to adjust the SVV, regardless of significant visual or somatosensory external information.

In addition, Mazibrada et al.2 found no significant bias of static SVV in three patients with bilateral somatosensory deafferentation. Furthermore, Trousselard et al.16 found that somatosensory inputs were required during body tilt to estimate the SVV and the body orientation. This finding suggests that proprioceptive afferents are more important when the head and body are tilted in order to influence the A- and E-effect. Moreover, it provides evidence for the use of a simple procedure that ensure the fixation of the head for the correct assessment of the visual vertical perception, with no haptic influence. Further studies are necessary to prove not only the influence of proprioceptive stimuli in the perception of visual verticality, but also to check the consequences of that influence in patients with vestibular or neurological dysfunction.

Received 7 October 2010

Accepted 9 February 2011

Support CAPES (Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior) and CNPq (Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico)

  • 1. Anastasopoulos D, Bronstein AM. A case of thalamic syndrome: somatosensory influences on visual orientation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:390-394.
  • 2. Mazibrada G, Tariq S, Pérennou D, Gresty M, Greenwood R, Bronstein AM. The peripheral nervous system and the perception of verticality. Gait Posture 2008;27:202-208.
  • 3. Barbieri G, Gissot AS, Foque F, Casillas JM, Pozzo T, Pérennou D. Does proprioception contribute to the sense of verticality? Exp Brain Res 2008; 185:545-552.
  • 4. Herdman SJ (Ed). Reabilitação Vestibular Rio de Janeiro: Ed. Manole; 2ª ed, 2005.
  • 5. Kanashiro AMK, Pereira, CB, Maia FM, Scaff M, Barbosa ER. Subjective visual vertical evaluation in normal Brazilian subjects. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2007;65:472-475.
  • 6. Kobayashi H, Hayashi Y, Higashino K, et al. Dynamic and static subjective visual vertical with aging. Auris Nasus Larynx 2002; 22:325-328.
  • 7. Pavan TZ, Colafemina JF, Tedeschi W, Carneiro AAO. Virtual apparatus to study yhe subjective visual vertical. International Summer School and Symposium on Medical Devices and Biosensors; Sept 4-6, 2006.
  • 8. Sharpe JA. What's up doc? Altered perception of the haptic, postural and visual vertical. Neurology 2003;61:1172-1173.
  • 9. Karnath HO, Ferber S, Dichgans J. The origin of contraversive pushing: evidence for a second graviceptive system in humans. Neurology 2000; 55:1298-1304.
  • 10. Perennou DA, Mazibrada G, Chauvineau V, et al. Lateropulsion, pushing and verticality perception in hemisphere stroke: a causal relationship? Brain 2008;131:2401-2413.
  • 11. Mittelstaedt H. Origin and processing of postural information. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1998;22:473-478.
  • 12. Anastopoulos D, Haswanter T, Bronstein A, Fetter M, Dichgans J. Dissociation between the perception of body verticality and the visual vertical in acute peripheral vestibular disorder in humans. Neurosci Lett 1997;233: 151-153.
  • 13. Trousselard M, Barraud PA, Nougier V, Raphel C, Cian C. Contribution of tactile and interoceptive cues to the perception of the direction of gravity. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2004;20:355-362.
  • 14. Curthoys IS, Dai MJ, Halmagyi GM. Human ocular torsional position before and after vestibular neurectomy. Exp Brain Res 1991;85:218-225.
  • 15. Saeys W, Vereeck L, Bedeer A, et al. Suppression of the E-effect during the subjective visual and postural vertical test in healthy subjects. Eur J Appl Physiol 2010;109:297-305.
  • 16. Trousselard M, Cian C, Nougier V, Pla S, Raphel C. Contribution of somesthetic cues to the perception of body orientation and subjective visual vertical. Percept Psychophys 2003;65:1179-1187.
  • 17. Bronstein AM. The interaction of otolith and proprioceptive information in the perception of verticality: the effects of labyrinthine and CNS disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1999;871:324-333.
  • 18. Faralli M, Longari F, Ricci G, Ibba MC, Frenguelli A. Influence of extero-and proprioceptive afferents of the plantar surface in determining subjective visual vertical in patients with unilateral vestibular dysfunction. Acta Otorhinolaryngol Ital 2009;29:245-250.
  • 19. Vibert D, Häusler R, Safran AB. Subjective visual vertical in peripheral unilateral vestibular diseases. J Vestib Res 1999;9:145-152.
  • 20. Kumagami H, Sainoo Y, Fujiyama D, et al. Subjective Visual Vertical in acute attacks of Ménière's disease. Otol Neurotol 2009;30:206-209.
  • 21. Kumagami H, Saino Y, Baba A, Fujiyama D, Takasaki K, Takahashi H. Subjective visual vertical test in patients with chronic dizziness without abnormal findings in routine vestibular function tests. Acta Otolaryngol 2009;129:46-49.
  • Correspondence:
    Taiza E.G. Santos-Pontelli
    Department of Neuroscience and Behavior - University of São Paulo School of Medicine at Ribeirão Preto
    14049-900 Ribeirão Preto SP - Brasil
    E-mail:
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      19 July 2012
    • Date of issue
      June 2011

    History

    • Received
      07 Oct 2010
    • Accepted
      09 Feb 2011
    Academia Brasileira de Neurologia - ABNEURO R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices Torre Norte, 04101-000 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: revista.arquivos@abneuro.org